Loris tardigradusslender loris

Ge­o­graphic Range

Loris tardi­gradus (the slen­der loris) is a strep­sirhine pri­mate re­stricted to the is­land of Sri Lanka. (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Phillips, 1980; Schulze and Meier, 1995a)

Habi­tat

Three sub­species of L. tardi­gradus and one sub­species of its only con­gener, Loris ly­dekke­ri­anus, main­tain fairly dis­crete habi­tat niches in Sri Lanka. Loris tardi­gradus tardi­gradus is found in wet, low­land forests of the south­west. Loris tardi­gradus gran­dis oc­cu­pies the cen­tral hills rang­ing from 731 m to 1036 m in el­e­va­tion and is thought to in­ter­breed with L. t. tardi­gradus at mid­dle el­e­va­tions in the ex­treme west of its range. Loris tardi­gradus nyc­tice­boides is a rare mon­tane form known only from its type lo­cal­ity in mist forests of the Hor­ton Plains at el­e­va­tions of greater than 1500 m. A sub­species of gray slen­der loris, Loris ly­dekke­ri­anus nordi­cus, in­hab­its the low­land dry forests and scrub jun­gle of the north. Some au­thors con­sider gran­dis and nyc­tice­boides to be sub­species of Loris ly­dekke­ri­anus (see Groves, 1998). This ac­count fol­lows the tax­o­nomic ac­count in Camp­bell et al. (2011). (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Groves, 1998; Phillips, 1980; Schulze and Meier, 1995a)

  • Range elevation
    0 to 2134 m
    0.00 to 7001.31 ft

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

Loris tardi­gradus is a small (127 g - 256 g) pri­mate with long, gracile limbs, a slen­der body, and no tail. Its face is char­ac­ter­ized by promi­nent ears, a short ros­trum, and large, for­ward-ori­ented eyes with hazel-brown irises. Dis­tinc­tive patches of dark fur sur­round the eyes, and a white stripe runs from fore­head to nose. The col­oration of cir­cu­m­oc­u­lar patches and the shape of the in­te­roc­u­lar stripe are used to dis­tin­guish sub­species. The ros­trum is sharply pointed and ends in a moist, naked rhi­nar­ium. Slen­der loris ears (both L. tardi­gradus and L. ly­dekke­ri­anus), are longer than those of slow lorises (genus Nyc­tice­bus), and the mar­gins of the ears are usu­ally hair­less. Fe­males have two pairs of mam­mae which are naked dur­ing lac­ta­tion and cov­ered with fur at all other times. Limbs are sube­qual as the legs are bulkier and slightly longer than the arms. The palms of the hands and the soles of the feet are usu­ally naked. As in other strep­sirhine pri­mates, there is a toi­let-claw on the sec­ond digit of both feet. Their hands are smaller than their feet, and the index fin­ger is re­duced. Dig­its close syn­chro­nously, with the hal­lux and pollex op­pos­able to dig­its two through five. Like Nyc­tice­bus, and in con­trast to Per­o­d­icti­cus, Loris species have a hair­less brachial gland on the in­side of the arms. This gland pro­duces an ex­u­date im­por­tant in ol­fac­tory com­mu­ni­ca­tion, which may also have an anti-preda­tor func­tion. The basal meta­bolic rate of Loris tardi­gradus is not known, but lorises in gen­eral have slower me­tab­o­lisms than would be ex­pected from their body size. Re­search in­di­cates a BMR of 0.38 (ml O2/g * hr) for their close rel­a­tive, L. ly­dekke­ri­anus mal­abar­i­cus. (Al­ter­man, 1995; Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Hagey, et al., 2007; Izard and Ras­mussen, 1998; Mac­don­ald, 2001; McNab, 1984; Müller, et al., 1985; Napier and Napier, 1967; Nekaris and Stevens, 2007; Nowak, 1999; Phillips, 1980; Schulze and Meier, 1995a; Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

Three sub­species of L. tardi­gradus are rec­og­nized and can be dis­tin­guished by size and pelage char­ac­ter­is­tics. Loris tardi­gradus tardi­gradus is the small­est slen­der loris, weigh­ing from 128 g to 142 g and with a head-body length not ex­ceed­ing 206 mm. Dor­sal pelage is red to red­dish-brown, dark­est on the shoul­ders and paler on the lower back and pelvis. The fur of the lower back may be slightly frosted, but less so than Loris tardi­gradus gran­dis. A dark-col­ored dor­sal stripe is some­times pre­sent, but never as con­spic­u­ous as that of Loris ly­dekke­ri­anus nordi­cus. Ven­tral fur is yel­low­ish. Patches around the eyes are chest­nut col­ored, and the in­te­roc­u­lar stripe rarely bi­fur­cates above the eyes. Fe­males are larger than males and have a brown color, with lit­tle or no red. In­fants are gray dor­sally and white ven­trally. Subadult males have fe­male-like col­oration and grad­u­ally be­come more red. Loris tardi­gradus gran­dis is larger than L. t. tardi­gradus, has a less del­i­cate ap­pear­ance, and is more heav­ily furred, es­pe­cially on the limbs. It weighs up to 227 g and has a head-body length be­tween 209 mm and 256 mm. Dor­sal fur is gray and ven­tral fur is white. Frost­ing is much more pro­nounced than in L. t. tardi­gradus. The cir­cu­m­oc­u­lar patches are black or dark brown, and the in­te­roc­u­lar stripe bi­fur­cates and merges into white fur sur­round­ing the ears. Fe­males are big­ger, darker, and more frosted than males. Ju­ve­niles are brown and do not have frost­ing. Loris tardi­gradus nyc­tice­boides is known only from a few spec­i­mens. Its mean weight is 140 g, and its head-body length ranges from 204 mm - 213 mm. The fur is con­sid­er­ably longer than that of the other sub­species, with ven­tral hairs as long as 30 mm. Dor­sal pelage is brown and the ven­tral pelage is buff. Patches around the eyes are black, and the nar­row in­te­roc­u­lar stripe bi­fur­cates and con­tin­ues around the eye patches. This sub­species is unique in that the ears are com­pletely furred. The hands and feet are also no­tably hairier than those of the other sub­species. (Phillips, 1980; Schulze and Meier, 1995a)

  • Sexual Dimorphism
  • female larger
  • sexes colored or patterned differently
  • Range mass
    127 to 227 g
    4.48 to 8.00 oz
  • Range length
    185 to 256 mm
    7.28 to 10.08 in
  • Average basal metabolic rate
    0.714 W
    AnAge

Re­pro­duc­tion

Lit­tle is known of the mat­ing habits of Loris tardi­gradus in the wild. A num­ber of be­hav­iors de­scribed in L. ly­dekke­ri­anus have not been con­firmed in L. tardi­gradus, and much of the avail­able data (for ei­ther taxon) comes from cap­tive in­di­vid­u­als. Both species are re­ported to form so­cial as­so­ci­a­tions in which the larger home ranges of one or more males over­lap the smaller range of a sin­gle fe­male. Loris tardi­gradus tardi­gradus has been ob­served to gather in semi-sta­ble sleep­ing groups of one fe­male, her off­spring, and a sin­gle male; groups with mul­ti­ple males are re­ported for L. ly­dekke­ri­anus. In both taxa, the pres­ence of a post-cop­u­la­tion vagi­nal plug of hard­ened semen has been re­ported. This, along with elab­o­rate pe­nile mor­phol­ogy, has been taken as ev­i­dence of sperm com­pe­ti­tion and a multi-male breed­ing sys­tem. Nekaris (2003) ob­served sev­eral wild male L. ly­dekke­ri­anus ro­tat­ing among three es­trus fe­males, with each male sep­a­rately groom­ing each fe­male at dif­fer­ent times over the course of a sin­gle night. Nekaris (2003) pro­poses a multi-male, multi-fe­male (i.e., promis­cu­ous) mat­ing sys­tem. (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Dix­son, 1995; Izard and Ras­mussen, 1985; Mac­don­ald, 2001; Napier and Napier, 1967; Nekaris, 2003; Nowak, 1999; Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

Ob­ser­va­tions of cap­tive Loris ly­dekke­ri­anus pop­u­la­tions in­di­cate no re­pro­duc­tive sea­son­al­ity, but this may not be true in the wild. It has been sug­gested that Loris tardi­gradus breed bian­nu­ally, peak birth times for L. ly­dekke­ri­anus in India occur dur­ing April and May and from Oc­to­ber to De­cem­ber. May-De­cem­ber birthing sched­ules have been re­ported in Sri Lanka as well. Short-term field stud­ies found no ev­i­dence for sea­son­al­ity in wild pop­u­la­tions of ei­ther Loris spe­ices and doc­u­mented one in­stance of a fe­male L. ly­dekke­ri­anus in es­trus while her close neigh­bors car­ried in­fants. Testis of both taxa switch fre­quently be­tween the scro­tal and in­guinal (i.e., de­scended and un­de­scended) con­di­tions, and no pat­tern of male gen­i­tal change has been dis­cov­ered with re­spect to fe­male es­trus or time-of-year. En­large­ment of male gen­i­tals ap­pears to be af­fected by am­bi­ent tem­per­a­ture rather than sex­ual ac­tiv­ity, with en­large­ment oc­cur­ring dur­ing in­creased tem­per­a­tures. (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Izard and Ras­mussen, 1985; Nekaris, 2003; Nowak, 1999; Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

Courtship by male Loris tardi­gradus con­sists of a lengthy pur­suit of the fe­male, with threat vo­cal­iza­tions and ap­pease­ment vo­cal­iza­tions. It is not clear which role, threat­ener or ap­peaser, each gen­der as­sumes. Re­searchers have been un­able to ob­serve the en­tire courtship process, and no cop­u­la­tions have been wit­nessed. Mat­ing be­hav­ior in L. tardi­gradus ap­pears to be sim­i­lar to that in L. ly­dekke­ri­anus nordi­cus, which can last for five hours and is char­ac­ter­ized by ap­pease­ment vo­cal­iza­tions and ex­cited branch-shak­ing on the part of the male, and threat vo­cal­iza­tions by the fe­male. In cap­tive L. ly­dekke­ri­anus, when the fe­male is ready for cop­u­la­tion she com­mu­ni­cates her ac­cep­tance by adopt­ing a sus­pen­sory pos­ture (i.e., hang­ing quadrupedally on the un­der­side of a branch). In­tro­mis­sion lasts from two to six­teen min­utes and is con­cluded by a threat vo­cal­iza­tion from the fe­male. Both gen­ders lick their gen­i­tals after cop­u­la­tion. A sin­gle cop­u­la­tion of L. ly­dekke­ri­anus has been doc­u­mented in the wild, which was pre­ceded by an hour-long courtship pur­suit and oc­curred in a sus­pen­sory po­si­tion under a hor­i­zon­tal branch. Males in other trees ha­rassed the mat­ing pair, and cop­u­la­tion was twice in­ter­rupted while the focal male chased away his ri­vals. (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Izard and Ras­mussen, 1985; Nekaris, 2003; Nowak, 1999; Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

Ges­ta­tion in Loris tardi­gradus lasts 166 to 175 days, and fe­males give birth to a max­i­mum of two lit­ters per year that usu­ally con­sists of sin­gle off­spring. Oc­ca­sion­ally, a fe­male may give birth to twins. Fe­males are known to reen­ter es­trus while nurs­ing a pre­vi­ous lit­ter. In­fants are al­tri­cial, cling­ing to their moth­ers con­tin­u­ously for the first four weeks of life. After four weeks, young are placed in a shel­tered lo­ca­tion dur­ing ac­tive night­time pe­ri­ods. Wean­ing takes place around 185 days, by which time the ju­ve­niles have achieved adult size. (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Izard and Ras­mussen, 1985; Nekaris, 2003; Nowak, 1999; Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

  • Breeding interval
    Loris tardigradus breeds twice a year
  • Breeding season
    None confirmed
  • Range number of offspring
    1 to 2
  • Average number of offspring
    1.3
    AnAge
  • Range gestation period
    167 to 174 days
  • Average weaning age
    185 days
  • Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    Sex: female
    380 days
    AnAge
  • Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    Sex: male
    365 days
    AnAge

Lit­tle in­for­ma­tion ex­ists con­cern­ing re­pro­duc­tion in slen­der lorises. The en­tire process, from cop­u­la­tion to in­de­pen­dence of off­spring, takes at least 320 days, and fe­males may be­come preg­nant with a new lit­ter be­fore wean­ing pre­vi­ous young. Phys­i­o­log­i­cal costs of lac­ta­tion are high, as fe­males pro­duce milk with un­usu­ally high fat and pro­tein con­tent com­pared to that of other strep­sirhine pri­mates. Sex­u­ally ma­ture fe­males make a sub­stan­tial caloric in­vest­ment in re­pro­duc­tion through­out the year, but more re­search is needed to quan­tify this in­vest­ment and to ex­plore its im­pli­ca­tions for feed­ing ecol­ogy, so­cial sys­tems, and pat­terns of parental care. (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Izard and Ras­mussen, 1985; Izard and Ras­mussen, 1998; Nekaris, 2003; Schulze and Meier, 1995b; Tilden and Of­tedal, 1997)

At birth, Loris tardi­gradus in­fants are help­less. They in­stinc­tively cling to their mother's fur and re­main there day-and-night for at least four weeks. After four weeks, lorises "park" their young in dense tan­gles of branches dur­ing night­time for­ag­ing ac­tiv­ity. Re­search in­di­cates that L. ly­dekke­ri­anus, a close rel­a­tive of L. tardi­gradus, moth­ers aban­doned their off­spring for the en­tire night, and that parked in­fants were some­times played-with and groomed by males. Al­though the re­la­tion­ship of males to in­fants is un­known, in­di­vid­u­als who babysat in this way be­longed to the same sleep­ing group as the mother. This be­hav­ior is prob­a­bly a form of af­fil­ia­tive pa­ter­nal care. The as­sis­tance of males in night­time par­ent­ing may give lac­tat­ing moth­ers in­creased mo­bil­ity when for­ag­ing, help­ing them to sat­isfy their high caloric ex­penses. In con­trast, L. tardi­gradus in­fants are not vis­ited by males, and fe­males re­turned to parked young as fre­quently as once per hour dur­ing the night. (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Izard and Ras­mussen, 1985; Izard and Ras­mussen, 1998; Nekaris, 2003; Schulze and Meier, 1995b; Tilden and Of­tedal, 1997)

Rare cases of in­fan­ti­cide have been re­ported for cap­tive Loris tardi­gradus. Such in­stances are al­ways said to be ac­com­pa­nied by "en­vi­ron­men­tal stress", and do not ap­pear to be di­rected by males to­ward the off­spring of other males. There are no re­ports of in­fan­ti­cide in wild pop­u­la­tions of L. tardi­gradus. (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Izard and Ras­mussen, 1985; Izard and Ras­mussen, 1998; Nekaris, 2003; Schulze and Meier, 1995b; Tilden and Of­tedal, 1997)

  • Parental Investment
  • altricial
  • female parental care
  • pre-hatching/birth
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • provisioning
      • female
  • post-independence association with parents
  • extended period of juvenile learning

Lifes­pan/Longevity

There is no in­for­ma­tion avail­able on the longevity of Loris tardi­gradus in the wild. One cap­tive in­di­vid­ual re­port­edly lived 15.5 years. (Nowak, 1999)

Be­hav­ior

Slen­der lorises are noc­tur­nal and ar­bo­real. Their move­ments are fa­mously slow, fluid, and noise­less. Tests on cap­tive an­i­mals in­di­cate nor­mal speeds of about 0.59 m/sec. Lorises are, how­ever, ca­pa­ble of rapid climb­ing and noisy episodes of branch-shak­ing. A re­cent field study by Nekaris and Stevens (2007) found "rapid quadrupedal­ism" to be sur­pris­ingly com­mon in slen­der lorises. This scram­bling mode of lo­co­mo­tion is qual­i­ta­tively dif­fer­ent from hand-over-hand climb­ing, and is used about 26% of the time. Dur­ing this form of move­ment, in­di­vid­u­als reach speeds ap­proach­ing 1.2 m/s. Slen­der lorises may be ca­pa­ble of "mini leaps", but they usu­ally move from tree to tree by care­ful can­tilever­ing, which in­volves grasp­ing a ver­ti­cal branch with their hindlimbs and ex­tend­ing its body hor­i­zon­tally across the gap. Hip, ankle, and wrist joints are very mo­bile (Napier & Napier, 1967), and pre­car­i­ous pos­tures can be main­tained for ex­tended pe­ri­ods be­cause retia mirabilia sup­ply the limb mus­cles with oxy­gen and re­move cel­lu­lar waste, pre­vent­ing cramp­ing. Lorises typ­i­cally travel on the tops of branches, and they pre­fer climb­ing struc­tures (e.g., branches or vines) small enough to be grasped in their hands. In­di­vid­u­als in a cap­tive grey slen­der loris colony showed ex­treme care when ne­go­ti­at­ing smooth ver­ti­cal trunks 10 cm in di­am­e­ter. In the same colony, bipedal stand­ing to the tops of branches and bipedal hang­ing by the hindlimbs un­der­neath branches were both re­peat­edly ob­served, how­ever, bipedal hang­ing by the fore­limbs was rare. (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Napier and Napier, 1967; Nekaris and Stevens, 2007; Nowak, 1999; Phillips, 1980; Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

Dur­ing the day, slen­der lorises ag­gre­gate in sleep­ing groups which in­clude a sin­gle adult fe­male, her off­spring, and one or more males. These as­so­ci­a­tions may be sta­ble from night to night, and adult males in a sleep­ing group may par­tic­i­pate in parental care. Both in cap­tiv­ity and in the wild, slen­der lorises par­tic­i­pate in so­cial co­he­sion be­hav­iors such as hud­dling, al­logroom­ing, play-wrestling and play-bit­ing. A va­ri­ety of vo­cal­iza­tions, in ad­di­tion to body lan­guage and elab­o­rate ol­fac­tory com­mu­ni­ca­tion are uti­lized. In cap­tiv­ity, grey slen­der lorises openly steel food from each other with­out pro­vok­ing hos­til­ity. Al­though ag­gres­sion cap­tive pop­u­la­tions is not un­com­mon, most ag­gres­sive be­hav­ior in the wild has been de­scribed be­tween un­re­lated adult fe­males and be­tween males who do not be­long to the same sleep­ing group. Night­time ac­tiv­i­ties re­ported for slen­der lorises con­sists pri­mar­ily of travel and for­ag­ing, but also in­cludes so­cial ac­tiv­i­ties, in­clud­ing those be­tween mother and young, and rest. (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Nekaris, 2003; Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

  • Range territory size
    15,900 to 36,000 m^2

Home Range

Al­though no in­for­ma­tion is avail­able con­cern­ing home range in Loris tardi­gradus, av­er­age home range for L. ly­dekke­ri­anus males is 3.6 ha (36,000 m^2) and 1.59 ha (15,900 m^2) for fe­males. (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Nekaris and Stevens, 2007)

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

There is no in­for­ma­tion avail­able re­gard­ing com­mu­ni­ca­tion and per­cep­tion in slen­der lorises.

Slen­der lorises call to one other through­out the night, and on sev­eral oc­ca­sions even di­rected vo­cal­iza­tions at po­ten­tial preda­tors. The known vocal reper­toire of slen­der lorises con­sists whis­tles, chit­ters, zic calls, krik calls, growls, and screams. Whis­tle in­di­cate ex­cite­ment and ag­gres­sion and con­sists of one to three dis­tinct syl­la­bles, each end­ing with a de­scend­ing fre­quency sweep. Uti­lized by males and fe­males, whis­tles are al­ways loud, and may be au­di­ble to hu­mans from 100 m away. Ev­i­dence sug­gests that whis­tles are most preva­lent in wild pop­u­la­tions, pos­si­bly be­cause long dis­tance com­mu­ni­ca­tions are un­nec­es­sary in cap­tiv­ity. In cap­tiv­ity, whistling in one cage pro­voked re­sponse vo­cal­iza­tions from groups caged nearby. Chit­ter are used as a de­fen­sive threat and are some­times as­so­ci­ated with star­ing or phys­i­cal shov­ing. Pri­mar­ily ut­tered by fe­males, chit­ters are often used in re­sponse to a courtship pur­suit by a male. Chit­ters con­sist of rapid clicks at fre­quen­cies up to 20 kHz. Zic calls con­sist of high fre­quency mono­syl­la­bles that are used by an in­fant to at­tract the at­ten­tion of its mother. Parked in­fants often zic-call, at which point the mother re­turns from for­ag­ing and col­lects the in­fant. Low in­ten­sity zic calls may be uti­lized when an in­fant dis­likes groom­ing, but high in­ten­sity calls sig­nify fear or pain. In cap­tiv­ity, adults other than the mother may com­fort a zic-call­ing in­fant. Krik calls are a hiss-like, low fre­quency sound used by males to ap­pease chit­ter­ing fe­males and are com­monly used by males as a pre­lude to al­logroom­ing. Fe­males use krick calls to ap­pease zic-call­ing in­fants. Some­times male-fe­male duets are per­formed using krik calls. Growl are an un­voiced threat vo­cal­iza­tion that are used against preda­tors, or by cap­tured an­i­mals. Rarely ut­tered in in­traspe­cific con­texts, growls are most often used in con­nec­tion with de­fen­sive body lan­guage. Screams are used in cir­cum­stances of pro­longed threat and are as­so­ci­ated with se­cre­tion of ex­u­date from the brachial gland. (Nekaris, 2003; Nekaris, et al., 2007; Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

Like many mam­mals and most noc­tur­nal pri­mates, slen­der lorises make ex­ten­sive use of scent mark­ings to com­mu­ni­cate in­for­ma­tion. Urine mark­ing are made by rhyth­mic mic­turi­tion (a stereo­typed be­hav­ior in which the an­i­mal trav­els along a branch and de­posits urine at in­ter­vals by rhyth­mi­cally low­er­ing its gen­i­tals to the sub­strate), anogen­i­tal drag­ging, and in­di­rect ap­pli­ca­tion (urine ap­plied to the sub­strate via the hands or feet of the uri­nat­ing in­di­vid­ual). Self-wash­ing with urine is reg­u­larly ob­served in a va­ri­ety of con­texts, in­clud­ing be­fore groom­ing in­fants prior to nightly park­ing, when prepar­ing to catch nox­ious in­sects, prior to con­sum­ing nox­ious prey items, and in re­sponse to being stung by them. Urine mark­ing is rarely ob­served in the vicin­ity of trees used for sleep­ing, but marks at lo­cal­i­ties within the range-over­lap area shared by sleep­ing-group mem­bers were ea­gerly re­ceived and coun­ter­marked. Scent com­mu­ni­ca­tion also plays a role in im­me­di­ate so­cial be­hav­ior. Anogen­ti­cal sniff­ing (male to fe­male) often pre­cedes groom­ing, and groom­ing bouts fre­quently in­volve rub­bing and lick­ing of the brachial gland. (Al­ter­man, 1995; Nekaris and Stevens, 2007; Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

Slen­der lorises use a va­ri­ety of com­mu­nica­tive pos­tures, ac­tions, and fa­cial ex­pres­sions in­di­cat­ing ag­gres­sion, sub­mis­sion, con­tent­ment, fear, sex­ual in­ten­tion (male), sex­ual readi­ness (fe­male), and other kinds of so­cially rel­e­vant in­for­ma­tion. Through al­logroom­ing and hud­dling, touch plays an im­por­tant role in es­tab­lish­ing and main­tain­ing group co­he­sion. It may also help ori­ent them in the dark. (Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

Food Habits

Slen­der lorises are pri­mar­ily in­sec­tiv­o­rous, spe­cial­iz­ing to some ex­tent on toxic and un­sa­vory species. Par­tic­u­larly nox­ious in­sects are eaten with ev­i­dent dis­taste, and in­sects which spray ir­ri­tant chem­i­cals are re­moved from their colonies and eaten some dis­tance away, while the loris sali­vates gra­tu­itously, shakes its head, and shuts its eyes. Ev­i­dence sug­gests that volatile chem­i­cals in­gested with in­sect food might be reused by lorises as a kind of ol­fac­tory cam­ou­flage. For­ag­ing lorises are fre­quently ob­served wash them­selves in urine be­fore ap­proach­ing toxic in­sects, per­haps in order to mask their own scent. The hunt­ing style of slen­der lorises and other lorises is to ap­proach silently and cau­tiously, then to make a sud­den ex­plo­sive grab with one or both hands. Pre­da­tion be­hav­ior of slen­der lorises has been de­scribed as a fixed ac­tion pat­tern that be­gins with vi­sual fix­a­tion of the prey, fol­lowed by lay­ing back of the loris' ears, and con­clud­ing with the grasp­ing mo­tion, which is al­ways com­pleted re­gard­less of whether or not the prey item is re­moved. In the wild, nearly 100% of the diet is pro­teina­ceous and in­cludes in­sects, tree frogs, geckos, small birds and eggs. They may oc­ca­sion­ally for­age on fruit when avail­able. In cap­tiv­ity, slen­der lorises are fed green sal­ads and plan­tains, and read­ily con­sume mice. (Camp­bell, et al., 2011; Mac­don­ald, 2001; Napier and Napier, 1967; Nekaris and Stevens, 2007; Nekaris, et al., 2007; Nowak, 1999; Phillips, 1980; Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

  • Animal Foods
  • birds
  • mammals
  • amphibians
  • reptiles
  • eggs
  • insects
  • Plant Foods
  • leaves
  • fruit
  • flowers

Pre­da­tion

There are few known preda­tors of Loris tardi­gradus. They may oc­ca­sion­ally be op­por­tunis­ti­cally taken by mam­malian car­ni­vores in­clud­ing fe­lids and rap­tors. It has been re­ported that genets and civets prey on lorises, but in sev­eral in­ter­ac­tions ob­served by re­searchers be­tween lorises and civets (Viver­ric­ula in­dica ma­jori, Para­dox­u­rus zey­lo­nen­sis) or cats (Felis viver­ri­nas), lorises sim­ply whis­tled until the po­ten­tial preda­tor moved away. It is not clear if the whis­tle func­tions as a warn­ing to con­specifics, or as a pur­suit de­ter­rence sig­nal to the preda­tor. Loris tardi­gradus fe­males and their in­fants react with alarm to the pres­ence of ven­omous com­mon kraits and moved to a shel­tered lo­ca­tion until the snake has passed. When cap­tured by re­searchers, lorises per­form a de­fen­sive be­hav­ior which re­sem­bles that of in­dian co­bras, which are pre­sent through­out the ge­o­graphic range of L. tardi­gradus. When cap­tured, L. tardi­gradus raises its arms above its head and sways its slen­der body side to side. (Al­ter­man, 1995; Hagey, et al., 2007; Krane, et al., 2003; Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

The brachial glands of slow lorises (Nyc­tice­bus) se­crete volatile chem­i­cals which can be toxic to hu­mans. It is not clear whether these com­pounds func­tion as a poi­son or an alarm pheromone, but se­cre­tion of a pun­gent-smelling ex­u­date from the brachial gland is a com­mon re­sult of fear in both slow and slen­der lorises. Al­ter­man (1995) hy­poth­e­sized that volatiles in the brachial gland do not be­come ac­tive until they are mixed with saliva, and per­formed pre­lim­i­nary tests sug­gest­ing that mam­malian car­ni­vores such as clouded leop­ards, bin­tur­ongs, and sun bears re­spond with ex­treme aver­sion to brachial gland ex­u­date when it is mixed with loris saliva, but not when it is pre­sented alone. Other re­searchers iden­ti­fied pro­teins in the brachial ex­u­date with re­gions of 70% se­quence sim­i­lar­ity to a known felid al­ler­gen. No stud­ies have in­ves­ti­gated the com­po­si­tion or func­tion of brachial gland ex­u­date in slen­der lorises, but L. tardi­gradus may be pro­tected from pre­da­tion by virtue of un­palata­bil­ity. (Al­ter­man, 1995; Hagey, et al., 2007; Krane, et al., 2003; Schulze and Meier, 1995b)

  • Anti-predator Adaptations
  • mimic

Ecosys­tem Roles

There is no in­for­ma­tion avail­able re­gard­ing the eco­log­i­cal role of slen­der lorises. They prey heav­ily on in­sects, but they are not com­mon any­where and it is doubt­ful that they con­trol pop­u­la­tions.

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Pos­i­tive

Slen­der lorises are il­le­gally hunted ex­ploited for tra­di­tional med­i­cine and for the pet trade. (Nekaris, 2010)

  • Positive Impacts
  • pet trade
  • body parts are source of valuable material

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Neg­a­tive

Loris bites may in­duce shock in hu­mans and are often slow to heal. How­ever, wild lorises are ex­tremely shy of con­tact and do not at­tack hu­mans un­less pro­voked. No other ad­verse ef­fects of Loris tardi­gradus on hu­mans are known. (Al­ter­man, 1995)

  • Negative Impacts
  • injures humans
    • bites or stings

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

Loris tardi­gradus is con­sid­ered en­dan­gered by the IUCN's Red List of Threat­ened Species and is listed under Ap­pen­dix II under the Con­ven­tion on In­ter­na­tional Trade in En­dan­gered Species of Wild Fauna and Flora. Major threats to their per­sis­tence in­clude habi­tat loss, hunt­ing for the pet trade and for their meat, road kills, su­per­sti­tious kills, and tra­di­tional med­i­cine. In his Man­ual of Sri Lankan mam­mals, W.W.A. Phillips wrote that L. tardi­gradus, "is most dif­fi­cult to dis­cover un­less the jun­gle in which it lives is being felled." Habi­tat de­struc­tion is a se­ri­ous threat to all three L. tardi­gradus sub­species, and the sur­vival of the species de­pends on the en­force­ment of their pro­tec­tions through­out 7 dif­fer­ent na­ture pre­serves in Sri Lanka and the es­tab­lish­ment of cor­ri­dors be­tween pro­tected areas. ("Ap­pen­dices I, II, and III", 2010; Nekaris, 2010; Phillips, 1980)

Con­trib­u­tors

Rory McGuin­ness (au­thor), Uni­ver­sity of Michi­gan-Ann Arbor, Phil Myers (ed­i­tor), Uni­ver­sity of Michi­gan-Ann Arbor, John Berini (ed­i­tor), An­i­mal Di­ver­sity Web Staff.

Glossary

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

altricial

young are born in a relatively underdeveloped state; they are unable to feed or care for themselves or locomote independently for a period of time after birth/hatching. In birds, naked and helpless after hatching.

arboreal

Referring to an animal that lives in trees; tree-climbing.

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

carnivore

an animal that mainly eats meat

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

duets

to jointly display, usually with sounds in a highly coordinated fashion, at the same time as one other individual of the same species, often a mate

endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

female parental care

parental care is carried out by females

forest

forest biomes are dominated by trees, otherwise forest biomes can vary widely in amount of precipitation and seasonality.

insectivore

An animal that eats mainly insects or spiders.

island endemic

animals that live only on an island or set of islands.

iteroparous

offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

nocturnal

active during the night

oriental

found in the oriental region of the world. In other words, India and southeast Asia.

World Map

pet trade

the business of buying and selling animals for people to keep in their homes as pets.

pheromones

chemicals released into air or water that are detected by and responded to by other animals of the same species

rainforest

rainforests, both temperate and tropical, are dominated by trees often forming a closed canopy with little light reaching the ground. Epiphytes and climbing plants are also abundant. Precipitation is typically not limiting, but may be somewhat seasonal.

scent marks

communicates by producing scents from special gland(s) and placing them on a surface whether others can smell or taste them

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

social

associates with others of its species; forms social groups.

tactile

uses touch to communicate

terrestrial

Living on the ground.

tropical

the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.

visual

uses sight to communicate

viviparous

reproduction in which fertilization and development take place within the female body and the developing embryo derives nourishment from the female.

year-round breeding

breeding takes place throughout the year

Ref­er­ences

2010. "Ap­pen­dices I, II, and III" (On-line). Con­ven­tion on In­ter­na­tional Trade in En­dan­gered Species of Wild Fauna and Flora. Ac­cessed April 11, 2011 at http://​www.​cites.​org/​eng/​app/​appendices.​shtml.

Al­ter­man, L. 1995. Tox­ins and tooth­combs: po­ten­tial al­lospe­cific chem­i­cal de­fenses in Nyc­tice­bus and Per­o­d­icti­cus. Pp. 413-424 in L Al­ter­man, G Doyle, M Izard, eds. Crea­tures of the Dark: The Noc­tur­nal Prosimi­ans. New York: Plenum Press.

Camp­bell, C., A. Fuentes, K. MacK­in­non, S. Bearder, R. Stumpf. 2011. Pri­mates in Per­spec­tive. Ox­ford: Ox­ford Uni­ver­sity Press.

Dix­son, A. 1995. Sex­ual se­lec­tion and the evo­lu­tion of cop­u­la­tory be­hav­ior in noc­tur­nal prosimi­ans. Pp. 95-188 in L Al­ter­man, G Doyle, M Izard, eds. Crea­tures of the Dark: The Noc­tur­nal Prosimi­ans. New York: Plenum Press.

Groves, C. 1998. Sys­tem­at­ics of tar­siers and lorises. Pri­mates, 39/1: 13-27.

Hagey, L., B. Fry, H. Fitch-Sny­der. 2007. Talk­ing de­fen­sively, a dual use for the brachial gland ex­u­date of slow and pygmy lorises. Pp. 253-272 in S Gursky, K Nekaris, eds. Pri­mate Anti-Preda­tor Strate­gies (De­vel­op­ments in Pri­ma­tol­ogy: Progress and Prospects). New York: Springer US.

Izard, M., D. Ras­mussen. 1985. Re­pro­duc­tion in the slen­der loris (Loris tardi­gradus mal­abar­i­cus). Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 8/2: 153-165.

Izard, M., D. Ras­mussen. 1998. Scal­ing of growth and life his­tory traits rel­a­tive to body size, brain size, and meta­bolic rate in lorises and gala­gos (Lorisidae, Pri­mates). Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Phys­i­cal An­thro­pol­ogy, 75/3: 357-367.

Krane, S., Y. Ita­gaki, K. Nakan­ishi, P. Wel­don. 2003. "Venom" of the slow loris: se­quence sim­i­lar­ity of prosimian skin gland pro­tein and Fel d 1 cat al­ler­gen. Nat­tur­wis­senschaften, 90: 60-62.

Mac­don­ald, D. 2001. The New En­cy­clo­pe­dia of Mam­mals. Ox­ford: Ox­ford Uni­ver­sity Press.

McNab, B. 1984. Phys­i­o­log­i­cal con­ver­gence amongst ant-eat­ing and ter­mite-eat­ing mam­mals. Jour­nal of Zo­ol­ogy, 204/4: 485-510.

Müller, E., U. Ni­eschalk, B. Meier. 1985. Ther­moreg­u­la­tion in the slen­der loris (Loris tardi­gradus). Folia Pri­ma­to­log­ica, 44/3-4: 216-226.

Napier, J., P. Napier. 1967. A Hand­book of Liv­ing Pri­mates: Mor­phol­ogy, Ecol­ogy and Be­hav­ior of Non­hu­man Pri­mates. New York: Aca­d­e­mic Press.

Nekaris, A. 2010. "Loris tardi­gradus" (On-line). IUCN Red List of Threat­ened Species. Ac­cessed April 11, 2011 at http://​www.​iucnredlist.​org/​apps/​redlist/​details/​12375/​0.

Nekaris, K. 2001. Ac­tiv­ity bud­get and po­si­tional be­hav­ior of the Mysore slen­der loris (Loris tardi­gradus ly­dekke­ri­anus): im­pli­ca­tions for slow climb­ing lo­co­mo­tion. Folia Pri­ma­to­log­ica, 72/4: 228-241.

Nekaris, K. 2003. Ob­ser­va­tions of mat­ing, birthing, and parental be­hav­ior in three sub­species of slen­der loris (Loris tardi­gradus and Loris ly­dekke­ri­anus) in India and Sri Lanka. Folia Pri­ma­to­log­ica, 74: 312-336.

Nekaris, K., E. Pim­ley, K. Ablard. 2007. Preda­tor de­fense by slen­der lorises and pot­tos. Pp. 222-240 in S Gursky, K Nekaris, eds. Pri­mate Anti-Preda­tor Strate­gies (De­vel­op­ments in Pri­ma­tol­ogy: Progress and Prospects). New York: Springer US.

Nekaris, K., D. Ras­mussen. 2003. Diet and feed­ing be­hav­ior of Mysore slen­der lorises. In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 24/1: 33-46.

Nekaris, K., N. Stevens. 2007. Not all lorises are slow: rapid ar­bo­real lo­co­mo­tion in Loris tardi­gradus of south­west­ern Sri Lanka. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 69/1: 112-120.

Nowak, R. 1999. Walker's Pri­mates of the World. Bal­ti­more: Johns Hop­kins Uni­ver­sity Press.

Phillips, W. 1980. Man­ual of the Mam­mals of Sri Lanka. Bat­tara­mulla: Wildlife and Na­ture Pro­tec­tion So­ci­ety of Sri Lanka.

Schulze, H., B. Meier. 1995. Be­hav­ior of cap­tive Loris tardi­gradus nordi­cus: a qual­i­ta­tive de­scrip­tion, in­clud­ing some in­for­ma­tion about mor­pho­log­i­cal bases of be­hav­ior. Pp. 221-250 in L Al­ter­man, G Doyle, M Izard, eds. Crea­tures of the Dark: The Noc­tur­nal Prosimi­ans. New York: Plenum Press.

Schulze, H., B. Meier. 1995. The sub­species of Loris tardi­gradus and their con­ser­va­tion sta­tus: a re­view. Pp. 193-210 in L Al­ter­man, G Doyle, M Izard, eds. Crea­tures of the Dark: The Noc­tur­nal Prosimi­ans. New York: Plenum Press.

Tilden, C., O. Of­tedal. 1997. Milk com­po­si­tion re­flects pat­terns of ma­ter­nal care in prosimian pri­mates. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 41/3: 195-211.