Spheniscus humboldtiHumboldt penguin

Ge­o­graphic Range

Hum­boldt pen­guins are en­demic to the sub­trop­i­cal Pa­cific coasts of Chile and Peru. Their range ex­tends from Isla Foca in the north to Puni­huil Is­lands to the south. This area is lo­cated ad­ja­cent to the Hum­boldt Cur­rent, a large oceanic up­welling char­ac­ter­ized by cold, nu­tri­ent rich wa­ters. (Culik, 2001)

Habi­tat

Hum­boldt pen­guins spend a ma­jor­ity of their time in coastal wa­ters. The amount of time the spend in water is as­so­ci­ated with breed­ing sta­tus. Non-breed­ing pen­guins spend an av­er­age of 60.0 hours in the water be­fore re­turn­ing to land with the max­i­mum trip last­ing 163.3 hours. Breed­ing pen­guins spend less time off­shore with trips av­er­ag­ing 22.4 hours with a max­i­mum of 35.3 hours. (Tay­lor, et al., 2004)

As is the case for many pen­guins, Hum­boldt pen­guins must come ashore to rest, breed, and raise young. The Pa­cific coast of South Amer­ica is char­ac­ter­is­ti­cally rocky and is also known for hav­ing large de­posits of guano. Pen­guins will choose such sites for nest­ing but will oc­ca­sion­ally use a cave along the shore. (Pare­des and Zavalaga, 2001)

  • Range depth
    61.8 (high) m
    202.76 (high) ft
  • Average depth
    10.1 m
    33.14 ft

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

Hum­boldt pen­guins are medium-sized, rang­ing from 66 to 70 cm in length and weigh­ing from 4 to 5 kg. They have black­ish-gray feath­ers on the back and white feath­ers on the chest. Hum­boldt pen­guins have black heads with white stripes under the eyes that wrap around the side of their head and con­nect at the chin form­ing a horse­shoe shape. Dis­tinc­tive of the species is a solid, black breast band that helps to dif­fer­en­ti­ate them from Mag­el­lanic pen­guins (Sphenis­cus mag­el­lan­i­cus). The solid breast band also helps in dis­tin­guish­ing adults from ju­ve­niles who have a darker head. (Berger, 2004)

  • Sexual Dimorphism
  • male larger
  • Average mass
    4 to 5 kg
    lb
  • Average length
    66 to 70 cm
    in

Re­pro­duc­tion

Hum­boldt pen­guins are monog­a­mous and rec­og­nize their part­ner in the colony through dis­tinct vocal cues. Al­though they are typ­i­cally monog­a­mous, in rare cases the fe­male will so­licit an­other male for mat­ing. Ex­tra-pair mat­ing is al­ways ini­ti­ated by the fe­male but the costs and ben­e­fits of this strat­egy are not well un­der­stood. (Schwarts, et al., 199)

An­other rare as­pect of the mat­ing of Hum­boldt pen­guins is that males may start usurps, de­fined as an un­paired male that in­vades the nest of an es­tab­lished pair. While this strat­egy is used to gain mates, there is risk of in­jury and even death of the in­vader. A sit­u­a­tion that might make usurpa­tion fa­vor­able in­cludes pe­ri­ods of low food avail­abil­ity that are char­ac­ter­is­tic of El Niño events. Re­search has shown that fe­males are dis­pro­por­tion­ately ef­fected at these times lead­ing to male-bi­ased sex ra­tios. With more males los­ing mates to mor­tal­ity, the rate of usurpa­tion may in­crease. (Tay­lor, et al., 2001)

Hum­boldt pen­guins can re­pro­duce nearly all year long due to the rel­a­tively warm on­shore cli­mate in the re­gion they oc­cupy. Breed­ing takes place be­tween March and De­cem­ber with peaks in April and Au­gust to Sep­tem­ber. Be­fore re­pro­duc­tion oc­curs pen­guins will molt. Dur­ing molt­ing, pen­guins stay on land and are in a fast­ing state for about two weeks. They will then go to sea to feed be­fore re­turn­ing to the breed­ing grounds to mate. (Pare­des, et al., 2002)

Hum­boldt pen­guins use cov­ered nests that pro­tect their eggs from both in­tense solar ra­di­a­tion, and aer­ial and ter­res­trial preda­tors. Of­ten­times pen­guins use the thick de­posits of guano found on the shore line to cre­ate their nests. They cre­ate bur­rows and de­posit their eggs safely in­side. Fe­males lay two, sim­i­lar sized eggs per clutch. After the eggs have been laid the male and fe­male will share the duty of at­tend­ing to the nest for the full ex­tent of the ap­prox­i­mately six week in­cu­ba­tion pe­riod. (Pare­des and Zavalaga, 2001)

  • Breeding interval
    Many Humboldt penguins breed successfully twice a year
  • Breeding season
    Humboldt penguins breed from March to December with peaks in April and August to September
  • Average eggs per season
    2
  • Range time to hatching
    40 to 42 days
  • Range fledging age
    10 to 12 weeks
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    2 (low) years
  • Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    3 years
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    2 to 3 years

As men­tioned pre­vi­ously, both sexes watch and pro­tect the nest be­fore hatch­ing oc­curs. After the eggs have hatched, the par­ents share in the re­spon­si­bil­ity of feed­ing the chicks. Adults must pro­vide enough food at fre­quent in­ter­vals for chick sur­vival but his must be bal­anced with self main­te­nance. This bal­ance is met by switch­ing be­tween short for­ag­ing trips in order to feed the chicks and longer trips for main­te­nance. Tay­lor et al. (2001) showed that pen­guins rais­ing chicks gen­er­ally for­age dur­ing the day and make short, shal­low dives. Once molt­ing oc­curs, which marks the be­gin­ning of the ju­ve­nile stage, off­spring will go into the ocean to for­age on their own and are fully in­de­pen­dent. (Tay­lor, et al., 2004)

  • Parental Investment
  • pre-hatching/birth
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female

Lifes­pan/Longevity

There is lit­tle known about the lifes­pan of Hum­boldt pen­guins. In­for­ma­tion from the wild is lack­ing, but Sea World re­ports that pen­guins live be­tween 15 to 20 years in their fa­cil­i­ties. It is thought that cap­tive pen­guins will live longer than wild pen­guins be­cause cap­tive pen­guins do not deal with preda­tors and also re­ceive a healthy, bal­anced diet. It is also noted that less than half of chicks make it past their first year of life.

  • Typical lifespan
    Status: captivity
    15 to 20 years

Be­hav­ior

Hum­boldt pen­guins typ­i­cally molt in Jan­u­ary. Stud­ies have shown that this be­hav­ior is cor­re­lated with a spike in thy­roid hor­mones at the same time that sex steroid hor­mones are at their low­est con­cen­tra­tion. Molt­ing is the process of re­plac­ing old worn feath­ers with new ones. For pen­guins this is ex­tremely im­por­tant as they need their feath­ers for in­su­la­tion and to seal water out. Over the two week pe­riod that molt­ing is oc­cur­ring, the pen­guins will fast be­cause they are un­able to enter the water to for­age. (El­len­berg, et al., 2006; Ot­suka, et al., 2003)

Hum­boldt pen­guins have been found to be ex­tremely sen­si­tive to human pres­ence. Their breed­ing suc­cess was sig­nif­i­cantly re­duced at sites that were highly vis­ited by tourists. Heart rate dras­ti­cally in­creased with the pres­ence of a human at 150 me­ters away and it took 30 min­utes for the pen­guin to re­cover. This sen­si­tiv­ity to hu­mans may be due to a his­tory of being hunted. (El­len­berg, et al., 2006; Ot­suka, et al., 2003)

Hum­boldt pen­guins live in large colonies and are con­sid­ered highly so­cial ex­cept dur­ing for­ag­ing. For­ag­ing be­hav­ior is de­pen­dent on whether the adult pen­guin is rais­ing chicks or not. Pen­guins not rais­ing fledg­lings are able to ex­plore dif­fer­ent for­ag­ing habi­tats that are fur­ther away be­cause they are able to be away from the colony for longer pe­ri­ods of time. Pen­guins rais­ing fledg­lings rarely take overnight for­ag­ing trips and gen­er­ally ex­hibit a shal­lower, shorter dive. (Tay­lor, et al., 2004)

Home Range

Satel­lite mon­i­tor­ing of free rang­ing Hum­boldt pen­guins shows has found that 90% of all satel­lite lo­ca­tions came from a range of 35 km around their breed­ing ground with max­i­mum dis­tances of 100 km. These val­ues in­crease dur­ing El Nino events. Pen­guins will aban­don their nests and may for­age up to 895 km off­shore. These re­sults con­tra­dicts an ear­lier hy­poth­e­sis that Hum­boldt pen­guins are pri­mar­ily seden­tary and re­main close to the breed­ing ground year round. (Culik and Luna-Jor­quera, 1997; Culik, 2001)

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

In­di­vid­u­als have vo­cal­iza­tions that are unique and dis­tin­guish­able by mates. Hum­boldt pen­guins dis­play three calls which in­clude the con­tact call, the dis­play call, and a threat call. In re­cent stud­ies, Hum­boldt pen­guins have been found to have an acute sense of smell. Odors cre­ate learned pref­er­ences to parental smells in chicks and are thought to play a role in so­cial and fam­ily in­ter­ac­tions. It is hy­poth­e­sized that be­cause of natal philopa­try, se­lec­tion fa­vors kin recog­ni­tion in order to avoid in­cest with sib­lings that were born in dif­fer­ent years and are there­fore un­fa­mil­iar. Hum­boldt pen­guins ex­plore un­fa­mil­iar scents but will re­turn to a fa­mil­iar scent and pre­fer it. There­fore, prior as­so­ci­a­tion of a scent likely al­lows for recog­ni­tion of nest-mates or colony mates. Fa­mil­iar scents may also be as­so­ci­ated with lo­cat­ing bur­rows at night. (Cof­fin, et al., 2011)

Hum­boldt pen­guins are be­lieved to use the same vi­sual mech­a­nisms as other pen­guins. They can­not per­ceive prey under low light con­di­tions. They ac­com­mo­date by de­creas­ing the ra­dius of cur­va­ture of the an­te­rior sur­face of their lens. They can see equally as well in the air and the water. The loss of corneal power under water is mit­i­gated by hav­ing a flat cornea and a spher­i­cal lens. This is also aided by ac­com­mo­da­tion. It was once be­lieved that pen­guins were my­opic in air but ev­i­dence from cur­rent stud­ies con­tra­dicts this. (Sivak, et al., 1987)

Food Habits

Hum­boldt pen­guins spe­cial­ize on pelagic schools of fish. Pen­guins lo­cated in the north­ern re­gions of Chile ate al­most ex­clu­sively garfish those found in cen­tral Chile pre­ferred an­chovy, pilchard, and squid. The dif­fer­ence is be­lieved to be due to the avail­abil­ity of the dif­fer­ent prey types in the par­tic­u­lar for­ag­ing re­gions. Ad­di­tion­ally, Hum­boldt pen­guins con­sume Arau­can­ian her­ring and sil­ver­sides. (Hen­nicke and Culik, 2005)

  • Animal Foods
  • fish

Pre­da­tion

In the ocean, Hum­boldt pen­guins are pre­dated on by sharks, fur seals, and sea lions. On land, the nests are preyed on by feral cats, dogs, foxes, snakes, and ro­dents. In some cases, these preda­tors at­tack a ju­ve­nile or adult Hum­boldt pen­guin if it is of smaller size. In­su­lar pen­guins lack anti-preda­tory tac­tics to rats and feral cats due to their re­cent in­tro­duc­tion of these preda­tors on is­lands by hu­mans. To a lesser ex­tent, gulls are also known to pre­date nests. Hum­boldt pen­guins use guano to cre­ate bur­rows for their eggs that help to re­duce ex­po­sure to preda­tors. The large colonies that Hum­boldt pen­guins live in also pro­vide de­fense by num­ber. In the water, their main de­fense is their abil­ity to swim quickly and with agility. (Sime­one and Guillermo, 2012)

Ecosys­tem Roles

Hum­boldt pen­guins are one of the main preda­tors in the Hum­boldt cur­rent sys­tem. (Her­ling, et al., 2005)

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Pos­i­tive

Hum­boldt pen­guins were con­sid­ered an im­por­tant source of guano. Guano is a rich fer­til­izer and a major source of in­come for the Pe­ru­vian gov­ern­ment. (Pare­des, et al., 2003)

In re­cent years, eco­tourism has begun to focus on the species. How­ever, Hum­boldt pen­guins are shy and acutely sen­si­tive to human pres­ence. In 2010, reg­u­la­tions were es­tab­lished to as­sist with breed­ing suc­cess while main­tain­ing tourist ac­tiv­ity. (El­len­berg, et al., 2006)

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Neg­a­tive

There are no known ad­verse ef­fects on hu­mans.

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

The major fac­tors con­tribut­ing to de­creases in Hum­boldt pen­guin pop­u­la­tions are fish­ery ac­tiv­ity and human dis­tur­bances. Pen­guins are at risk of en­tan­gle­ment and drown­ing in fish­ing nets while fish­eries threaten food avail­abil­ity. Guano ex­trac­tion has also re­duced the breed­ing suc­cess of pen­guins. Guano re­serves are being formed in order to re­duce this threat to pop­u­la­tion vi­a­bil­ity. (Pare­des, et al., 2003)

Con­trib­u­tors

Heidi Niss­ley (au­thor), In­di­ana Uni­ver­sity-Pur­due Uni­ver­sity Fort Wayne, Mark Jor­dan (ed­i­tor), In­di­ana Uni­ver­sity-Pur­due Uni­ver­sity Fort Wayne, Tanya Dewey (ed­i­tor), Uni­ver­sity of Michi­gan-Ann Arbor.

Glossary

Neotropical

living in the southern part of the New World. In other words, Central and South America.

World Map

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

carnivore

an animal that mainly eats meat

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

coastal

the nearshore aquatic habitats near a coast, or shoreline.

colonial

used loosely to describe any group of organisms living together or in close proximity to each other - for example nesting shorebirds that live in large colonies. More specifically refers to a group of organisms in which members act as specialized subunits (a continuous, modular society) - as in clonal organisms.

diurnal
  1. active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
ecotourism

humans benefit economically by promoting tourism that focuses on the appreciation of natural areas or animals. Ecotourism implies that there are existing programs that profit from the appreciation of natural areas or animals.

endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

iteroparous

offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).

monogamous

Having one mate at a time.

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

natatorial

specialized for swimming

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

oviparous

reproduction in which eggs are released by the female; development of offspring occurs outside the mother's body.

piscivore

an animal that mainly eats fish

saltwater or marine

mainly lives in oceans, seas, or other bodies of salt water.

seasonal breeding

breeding is confined to a particular season

sedentary

remains in the same area

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

tactile

uses touch to communicate

temperate

that region of the Earth between 23.5 degrees North and 60 degrees North (between the Tropic of Cancer and the Arctic Circle) and between 23.5 degrees South and 60 degrees South (between the Tropic of Capricorn and the Antarctic Circle).

terrestrial

Living on the ground.

threatened

The term is used in the 1994 IUCN Red List of Threatened Animals to refer collectively to species categorized as Endangered (E), Vulnerable (V), Rare (R), Indeterminate (I), or Insufficiently Known (K) and in the 1996 IUCN Red List of Threatened Animals to refer collectively to species categorized as Critically Endangered (CR), Endangered (EN), or Vulnerable (VU).

tropical

the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.

visual

uses sight to communicate

Ref­er­ences

Berger, C. 2004. Sphenis­ci­formes (Pen­guins). Pp. 147-158 in E Hutchins, A Evans, J Jack­son, D Kleiman, J Mur­phy, D Thoney, eds. Grz­imek's An­i­mal Life En­cy­clo­pe­dia, Vol. 8, 22 Edi­tion. De­troit, Michi­gan: Gale Vir­tual Ref­er­ence Li­brary.

Cof­fin, H., J. Wat­ters, J. Mateo. 2011. Odor-Based Recog­ni­tion of Fa­mil­iar and Re­lated Con­specifics: A First Test Con­ducted on Cap­tive Hum­boldt Pen­guins. PLoS ONE, 6.

Con­stan­tini, V., A. Guar­icci, P. Lar­ic­chuita, F. Rausa, G. La­ca­lan­dra. 2008. DNA Sex­ing in Hum­boldt Pen­guins from Feather Sam­ples. An­i­mal Re­pro­duc­tion Sci­ence, 106: 162-167.

Culik, B. 2001. Find­ing Food in the Open Ocean: For­ag­ing Strate­gies in Hum­boldt Pen­guins. Zo­ol­ogy, 104: 327-338.

Culik, B., G. Luna-Jor­quera. 1997. Satel­lite Track­ing of Hum­boldt Pen­guins Sphenis­cus hum­boldti in North­ern Chile. Ma­rine Bi­ol­ogy, 128: 547-556.

El­len­berg, U., T. Mat­tern, P. Sed­don, G. Luna Jorguera. 2006. Physi­co­log­i­cal and Re­pro­duc­tive Con­se­quences of Human Dis­tur­bances in Hum­boldt Pen­guins: The Need for Species-Spe­cific Vis­i­tor Man­age­ment. Bi­o­log­i­cal Con­ser­va­tion, 133: 95-106.

Hen­nicke, J., B. Culik. 2005. For­ag­ing Per­for­mance and Re­pro­duc­tive Suc­cess of Hum­boldt Pen­guins in Re­la­tion to Prey Avail­abil­ity. Ma­rine Ecol­ogy Progress Se­ries, 296: 173-181.

Her­ling, C., B. Culik, J. Hen­nicke. 2005. Diet of the Hum­boldt Pen­guin in North­ern and South­ern Chile. Ma­rine Bi­ol­ogy, 147: 13-25.

Ot­suka, R., T. Machida, M. Wada. 2003. Hor­monal Cor­re­la­tions at Tran­si­tion from Re­pro­duc­tion to Molt­ing in an An­nual Life Cycle of Hum­boldt Pen­guins. Gen­eral and Com­par­a­tive En­docrinol­ogy, 135: 175-185.

Pare­des, R., C. Zavalaga. 2001. Nest­ing Sites and Nest Types as Im­por­tant Fac­tors for the Con­ser­va­tion of Hum­boldt Pen­guins. Bi­o­log­i­cal Con­ser­va­tion, 100: 190-205.

Pare­des, R., C. Zavalaga, G. Bat­tis­tini, P. Ma­jluf, P. McGill. 2003. Sta­tus of the Hum­boldt Pen­guin in Peru, 199-2000. Wa­ter­bird So­ci­ety, 26: 129-138.

Pare­des, R., C. Zavalaga, D. Boness. 2002. Pat­terns of Egg Lay­ing and Breed­ing Suc­cess in Hum­boldt Pen­guins at Punta San Juan, Peru. The Auk, 119: 244-250.

Schwarts, M., D. Boness, C. Scha­eff, P. Ma­jluf, E. Perry. 199. Fe­male-So­licited Ex­tra­pair at­ings in Hum­boldt Pen­guins Fail to Pro­duce Ex­tra­pair Fer­til­iza­tions. Be­hav­ioral Ecol­ogy, 10: 242-250.

Sime­one, A., M. Bernal. 2000. Ef­fects of Habi­tat Mod­i­fi­ca­tion on Breed­ing Seabirds: A Case Study in Cen­tral Chile. Wa­ter­bird So­ci­ety, 23: 449-456.

Sime­one, A., L. Guillermo. 2012. Es­ti­mat­ing Rat Pre­da­tion on Hum­boldt Pen­guins in North-Cen­tral Chile. Jour­nal of Or­nithol­ogy, 153: 1079-1085.

Sivak, J., H. How­land, P. McGill-Harel­stad. 1987. Vi­sion of the Hum­boldt Pen­guin in Air and Water. Pro­ceed­ings of the Royal So­ci­ety of Lon­don. Se­ries B, Bi­o­log­i­cal Sci­ences, 229: 467-472.

Tay­lor, S., M. Leonard, D. Boness, P. Ma­jluf. 2004. Hum­boldt Pen­guins Change Their For­ag­ing Be­hav­ior Fol­low­ing Breed­ing Fail­ure. Ma­rine Orinthol­ogy, 32: 63-67.

Tay­lor, S., M. Leonard, D. Boness. 2001. Ag­gres­sive Nest In­tru­sions by Male Hum­boldt Pen­guins. The Con­dor, 103: 162-165.

Wal­lace, R., J. Dubach, M. Mar­garet, N. Keuler, E. Diebold, k. Grzy­bowski, A. Teare, M. Willis. 2008. Mor­pho­me­t­ric De­ter­mi­na­tion of Gen­der in Adult Hum­boldt Pen­guins. Wa­ter­bird So­ci­ety, 31: 448-453.