Papio anubisanubis baboon(Also: olive baboon)

Ge­o­graphic Range

Papio anu­bis is the most broadly dis­trib­uted ba­boon species, rang­ing through most of cen­tral sub-Sa­ha­ran Africa. Iso­lated pop­u­la­tions occur within the Sa­ha­ran re­gion.

This species is part of a com­plex of closely re­lated African ba­boon species. We have an ac­count of the whole genus under Papio. (Groves, 2001; Jolly, 1993; Nowak, 1999; Pri­mate Info Net, 2000)

Habi­tat

Anu­bis ba­boons are found in sa­van­nah, grass­land steppe, and rain­for­est habi­tats. (Jolly, 1993; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Nowak, 1999; Pri­mate Info Net, 2000)

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

These mon­keys are highly sex­u­ally di­mor­phic in size and pelage char­ac­ters. Males weigh around 25 kg and fe­males around 14 kg, with some ge­o­graphic vari­a­tion in av­er­age size. The head and body of the av­er­age male mea­sures 760 mm, with the tail adding an ad­di­tional 560 mm. Fe­males are smaller, with an av­er­age head and body mea­sure­ment of 600 mm and an av­er­age tail length of 480 mm. Males have large ca­nine teeth, whereas the teeth of the fe­males are much smaller. Pelage is char­ac­ter­is­ti­cally a dark, olive-gray. This over­all color is pro­duced by hairs with 1 to 2 al­ter­nat­ing pairs of black and yel­low-brown rings. Males pos­sess a large mane, re­stricted to the an­te­rior por­tion of the body, and grad­ing into the shorter body hair to­ward the rear. Fe­males lack a mane. (Groves, 2001; Jolly, 1993; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Napier and Napier, 1985; Nowak, 1999; Pri­mate Info Net, 2000)

The skin of the face and around the is­chial cal­losi­ties is dark gray to black in both sexes. The bare area of the rump is much smaller in this species than in Papio hamadryas or Papio papio. Un­like some species of ba­boons, the nos­trils of P. anu­bis point for­ward. The head is flat on top, help­ing to dis­tin­guish this species from Papio cyno­cephalus which has a promi­nent crest on the top of the head. The first quar­ter of the tail is car­ried erect, being held straight up­ward, with the re­main­der of the tail falling down limp, giv­ing the tail a bro­ken ap­pear­ance. The natal pelage is black, but this fur is re­placed by the typ­i­cal olive-gray by about 6 months of age. (Groves, 2001; Jolly, 1993; Napier and Napier, 1985; Nowak, 1999)

The skull of males has heavy ridges of bone on both sides of the nose, and a promi­nent rounded bar above the or­bits. There is a sagit­tal crest and often a nuchal crest. Mo­lars are large, and the first lower pre­mo­lar has been mod­i­fied into a hone for the upper ca­nine. These pri­mates have 32 teeth. (Al­berts and Alt­mann, 2001; Groves, 2001; Jolly, 1993; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Nowak, 1999)

  • Range mass
    14 to 25 kg
    30.84 to 55.07 lb
  • Range length
    480 to 760 mm
    18.90 to 29.92 in

Re­pro­duc­tion

Re­pro­duc­tive be­hav­ior in P. anu­bis is closely tied to so­cial or­ga­ni­za­tion. Be­cause these an­i­mals live in multi-male, multi-fe­male troops, there is the po­ten­tial for any male to mate with any fe­male. This re­sults in fierce com­pe­ti­tion be­tween males for ac­cess to sex­u­ally re­cep­tive fe­males. In gen­eral, a male’s abil­ity to con­sort with a fe­male and ex­clude other males from ac­cess to her is re­lated to the male’s abil­ity to com­pete with other males. There is there­fore a cor­re­la­tion be­tween male dom­i­nance rank within the troop and mat­ing suc­cess. Larger, younger, and stronger males have a dis­tinct ad­van­tage in this type of com­pe­ti­tion. (Smuts, 1987a)

How­ever, as in many so­cial an­i­mals, there are other fac­tors which af­fect a male’s mat­ing suc­cess. For ex­am­ple, males may form al­liances with other males, which can sub­vert the nor­mal dom­i­nance hi­er­ar­chy. Two males, nei­ther of whom can dom­i­nate a third male alone, may join forces and to­gether, as a coali­tion, these males may suc­ceed in se­cur­ing ac­cess to a sex­u­ally re­cep­tive fe­male. Such coali­tions are rec­i­p­ro­cal, and typ­i­cally occur be­tween pairs of older males who are well ac­quainted with one an­other through mu­tual tenure in a troop. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Smuts, 1987a)

Males also fol­low a strat­egy of de­vel­op­ing "friend­ships" with fe­males, which en­hances their op­por­tu­ni­ties to mate. In these friend­ships, males groom, share food, and have strong af­fil­ia­tive ties with par­tic­u­lar fe­males and their off­spring. It is com­mon for males to de­fend their fe­male friends dur­ing ag­o­nis­tic en­coun­ters with other fe­males, and with other males. These as­so­ci­a­tions are not con­fined to the pe­riod dur­ing which fe­males are sex­u­ally re­cep­tive, but span the en­tire gamut of the fe­male’s re­pro­duc­tive life—in­clud­ing preg­nancy, lac­ta­tion, and time spent cy­cling. Fe­males tend to ex­hibit a pref­er­ence for mat­ing with their male friends, and there­fore make con­sortships with their male friends more likely. In ad­di­tion, be­cause fe­males pre­fer their friends as mates, they are more likely to co­op­er­ate with them in the main­te­nance of a con­sortship than they are to co­op­er­ate with other, less fa­vored, males. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Smuts, 1987a)

The pro­longed pe­riod of sex­ual re­cep­tiv­ity of fe­males in this species is typ­i­cal of pri­mates liv­ing in multi-male, multi-fe­male so­cial groups. Fe­males in monog­a­mous or polyg­y­nous species are typ­i­cally re­cep­tive for a very short time around ovu­la­tion. In P. anu­bis, fe­males mate with a va­ri­ety of males over a pe­riod of 15 to 20 days. Such mul­ti­ple mat­ings are not nec­es­sary to en­sure fer­til­iza­tion, and may func­tion to con­fuse the ac­tual pa­ter­nity of the fe­male’s off­spring. This may help to mit­i­gate in­fan­ti­ci­dal ten­den­cies of males. (Hrdy and Whit­ten, 1987; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987)

Fe­males exert some mate choice in this species. By mak­ing con­sortships eas­ier or more dif­fi­cult for males, fe­males can exert some con­trol over whom they mate with. Also, fe­males can make it eas­ier or more dif­fi­cult for a given male to im­mi­grate into the troop, in­flu­enc­ing the pool of males from which they may choose mates.

Mat­ing is ini­ti­ated by the fe­male, who pre­sents her hindquar­ters to the male. The male mounts the fe­male and thrusts about 6 times, then ejac­u­lates. Mat­ings are prob­a­bly quick be­cause of the in­tense in­ter­male com­pe­ti­tion for ac­cess to sex­u­ally re­cep­tive fe­males. (Hrdy and Whit­ten, 1987; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Smuts, 1987b; Smuts, 1987a)

Re­pro­duc­tion in P. anu­bis is re­lated to the so­cial struc­ture of this species. Anu­bis ba­boons live in multi-male, multi-fe­male troops. Mat­ing is polyg­y­nan­drous, with both males and fe­males mat­ing with mul­ti­ple part­ners. Most mat­ings occur dur­ing con­sortships. Con­sortships arise when a male, through ag­gres­sion to­ward po­ten­tial ri­vals, is able to main­tain ex­clu­sive sex­ual ac­cess to a fe­male. Fe­males may con­sort with mul­ti­ple males while they are sex­u­ally re­cep­tive, al­though they con­sort with only one male at a time. Be­cause it is ap­par­ently eas­ier for a male to main­tain ex­clu­sive ac­cess to a fe­male if the fe­male is co­op­er­a­tive, there is a sig­nif­i­cant amount of fe­male mate choice, with fe­males pre­fer­ring some part­ners over oth­ers. (Hamil­ton III and Bul­ger, 1992; Hrdy and Whit­ten, 1987; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Smuts, 1987a)

Fe­males char­ac­ter­is­ti­cally have an es­trous cycle of 31 to 35 days in length. There is a no­tice­able men­strual flow for ap­prox­i­mately three days per cycle if the fe­male does not con­ceive. Dur­ing the pe­riod around ovu­la­tion, the per­ineal skin of the fe­male swells, and aliphatic acids are pro­duced, alert­ing the males to her po­ten­tially fer­tile con­di­tion, and en­hanc­ing her at­trac­tive­ness to them. Fe­males are typ­i­cally re­cep­tive for 15 to 20 days per cycle. (Hrdy and Whit­ten, 1987; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987)

Ges­ta­tion lasts about 180 days, after which the fe­male gives birth to a sin­gle off­spring, weigh­ing ap­prox­i­mately 1068 g. The neonate has a black coat, mak­ing it easy to dis­tin­guish from older in­fants. An in­fant is com­pletely de­pen­dent upon its mother for the first few months, until it be­gins to eat solid food and is able to walk on its own. Fe­males have an in­ter­birth in­ter­val rang­ing from 12 to 34 months. This in­ter­val varies ac­cord­ing to a num­ber of fac­tors. Fe­males who are older or have a higher rank tend to have shorter in­ter­birth in­ter­vals. In­ter­birth in­ter­val is also shorter if an in­fant dies be­fore wean­ing. (Bercov­itch, 1987; Hrdy and Whit­ten, 1987; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Smuts and Nicol­son, 1989; Strum, 1991)

Wean­ing typ­i­cally oc­curs around 420 days of age. Lac­ta­tion is a huge cost for adult fe­males, and typ­i­cally causes a re­duc­tion in fe­male weight. Lower rank­ing and younger fe­males prob­a­bly take longer to re­cover ad­e­quate body weight to re­pro­duce than do older, dom­i­nant fe­males, ex­plain­ing their longer in­ter­birth in­ter­vals. (Bercov­itch, 1987; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Smuts and Nicol­son, 1989)

The onset of pu­berty and at­tain­ment of adult size is highly vari­able and is as­so­ci­ated with nu­tri­tion lev­els. In pop­u­la­tions where ba­boons are known to raid human crops, and to thereby se­cure greater ac­cess to nu­tri­ents than nat­u­rally for­ag­ing an­i­mals, pu­berty can occur much ear­lier. In such food-sup­ple­mented pop­u­la­tions, males at­tain adult body weight be­tween 7 and 8 years. Fe­males reach full size by 6.5 years. In con­trast, in nat­u­rally for­ag­ing pop­u­la­tions, males do not reach full adult size until they are 7 to 10 years old, and fe­males do not reach adult weight until they are 7 to 8 years old. The ef­fect of nu­tri­tion on growth is so strong that as lit­tle as 15 to 16 weeks of di­etary vari­a­tion in new­borns can have last­ing ef­fects on over­all rates of fe­male growth, ab­solute adult weight, and age at menar­che. (Strum, 1991)

In nat­u­rally for­ag­ing pop­u­la­tions, pu­berty oc­curs be­tween the ages of 5 and 6 years in fe­males, and is sig­naled by menar­che, or in some cases, first preg­nancy. In males, pu­berty be­gins around 6.6 years of age, when body size be­gins to in­crease rapidly, third mo­lars erupt, and ca­nine teeth fully erupt. Changes in male body size in­clude in­creases in mus­cu­lar­ity, which give these an­i­mals a broader pro­file. The mane of males also be­gins to de­velop, con­tribut­ing to the in­crease in shoul­der size. Cor­re­lated with these changes in body size, male anu­bis ba­boons un­dergo an in­crease in tes­tic­u­lar vol­ume. Like human males un­der­go­ing pu­berty, male anu­bis ba­boons are also re­ported to un­dergo a break in their voices around this time, even­tu­ally lead­ing to a deeper sound­ing alarm-bark. Males typ­i­cally em­i­grate from their natal troop just after these changes are com­pleted. (Jolly and Phillips-Con­roy, 2003; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Strum, 1991; Wal­ters, 1987)

  • Breeding interval
    Female anubis baboons with ample food and health can breed annually. However the interbirth interval ranges between 12 and 34 months, because female condition and food supply vary.
  • Breeding season
    Mating can occur throughout the year.
  • Average number of offspring
    1
  • Average gestation period
    180 days
  • Average weaning age
    420 days
  • Average time to independence
    420 days
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    7 to 8 years
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    7 to 10 years

Most parental be­hav­ior is per­formed by the fe­male. Fe­males nurse, groom, and play with their off­spring. Fe­males ex­press dif­fer­ent pat­terns of in­fant care, often as­so­ci­ated with rank and age. In yel­low ba­boons, higher-rank­ing fe­males tend to be more "per­mis­sive" in their par­ent­ing than lower rank­ing fe­males, who tend to me more ner­vous and "re­stric­tive," pre­vent­ing their off­spring from mov­ing away from them. This dif­fer­ence has been re­ported in anu­bis ba­boons only up to the age of 8 weeks of in­fant life, but may be longer for some fe­males or in some troops. An­other dif­fer­ence seen in ma­ter­nal be­hav­ior in this species is that older moth­ers are known to spend more time in con­tact or close to their in­fants and are less likely to ter­mi­nate bouts of nurs­ing than are younger fe­males. First-time moth­ers are also likely to re­ject in­fants sooner than are ex­pe­ri­enced moth­ers. These dif­fer­ences may af­fect in­ter­birth in­ter­vals. (Alt­mann, 1980; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Nicol­son, 1987; Smuts and Nicol­son, 1989)

There does not seem to be co­op­er­a­tive care of off­spring among fe­males in P. anu­bis, but it is not un­com­mon for fe­males other than the mother to groom an in­fant, some­times pro­vid­ing al­lo­ma­ter­nal care to the in­fant. Subadult and ju­ve­nile fe­males who have not yet re­pro­duced them­selves are most likely to ex­hibit al­lo­ma­ter­nal be­hav­ior. As is the case for all ba­boons, in­fants are very at­trac­tive to other mem­bers of the so­cial group, and are the focus of a great deal of in­ves­ti­ga­tion and at­ten­tion, es­pe­cially while they are still dis­play­ing their black natal coat. In ex­treme cases, fe­males may kid­nap the off­spring of other fe­males. Lower-rank­ing fe­males are more often sub­ject to this ex­treme form of ha­rass­ment than are higher-rank­ing fe­males. Other fac­tors known to af­fect the in­ci­dence of al­lo­ma­ter­nal be­hav­ior in other species in­clude the in­fant’s age, and re­lat­ed­ness of the al­lo­mother to the mother and in­fant. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Nicol­son, 1987)

Males have com­plex re­la­tion­ships with in­fants and ju­ve­niles, which in some cases may be a form of parental care. Males are known to carry, pro­tect, share food (es­pe­cially meat), groom, and play with, the off­spring of their fe­male friends. Be­cause they are more likely to mate with their fe­male friends than they are with other fe­males, these in­fants and ju­ve­niles are more likely to be their own off­spring than are other im­ma­ture an­i­mals within the troop. This be­hav­ior, there­fore, can be in­ter­preted as pa­ter­nal. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Stein, 1984; Whit­ten, 1987)

How­ever, it should be noted that the re­la­tion­ship be­tween adult males and these im­ma­ture an­i­mals may be more com­plex than this. There may be some form of rec­i­proc­ity in­volved. Adult males will often carry in­fants dur­ing tense in­ter­ac­tions with other adult males. This car­ry­ing can be ini­ti­ated ei­ther by the adult male or by the in­fant. Such con­tact with an in­fant dur­ing ag­o­nis­tic en­coun­ters may have the ef­fect of in­hibit­ing ag­gres­sion by other males. The fa­vors be­stowed upon an in­fant used as a buffer may there­fore be a form a "pay­back" from the adult male. How­ever, since the ten­dency to use an in­fant as an ag­o­nis­tic buffer is re­lated to fa­mil­iar­ity with the in­fant and the prob­a­bil­ity of pa­ter­nity, it is im­pos­si­ble to sep­a­rate the nepo­tism from the rec­i­proc­ity of such in­ter­ac­tions. (Stein, 1984)

  • Parental Investment
  • altricial
  • pre-fertilization
    • provisioning
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-hatching/birth
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • post-independence association with parents
  • extended period of juvenile learning
  • inherits maternal/paternal territory
  • maternal position in the dominance hierarchy affects status of young

Lifes­pan/Longevity

The max­i­mum lifes­pan of a cap­tive hamadryas ba­boon is mea­sured at 37.5 years. A cap­tive chacma ba­boon is re­ported to have lived 45 years in cap­tiv­ity. Al­though the lifes­pan of P. anu­bis has not been re­ported, it is likely to be sim­i­lar to these two species, al­though some­what shorter than these in the wild. (Nowak, 1999)

Be­hav­ior

Ba­boons are quadrupedal, mainly ter­res­trial pri­mates. They are highly so­cial an­i­mals, with a com­plex multi-male, multi-fe­male so­cial struc­ture. Mem­bers of a troop travel, for­age, and sleep to­gether. An av­er­age troop may be com­prised of 39 to 97 an­i­mals. The move­ments of a troop may be lim­ited by the avail­abil­ity of ap­pro­pri­ate sleep­ing lo­ca­tions. Be­cause the troop beds down in trees, or on rocks/cliffs, ac­tiv­ity of the troop must be co­or­di­nated so that one of a set num­ber of sleep­ing sites can be reached by night­fall. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Nowak, 1999)

All males em­i­grate from their natal troops, with 85 per cent of males em­i­grat­ing prior to the time that they reach full adult size. Upon set­tling in a new troop, males must es­tab­lish them­selves in the male dom­i­nance hi­er­ar­chy of that troop. This typ­i­cally in­volves ag­gres­sive be­hav­ior be­tween males, with the "win­ner" of an en­counter es­tab­lish­ing dom­i­nance over the "loser." Some males may em­i­grate into new groups in pairs. These male pairs may be half - or full- sib­lings from the same natal troop. Aside from such oc­ca­sional life-long af­fil­i­a­tions, males do not main­tain long-term bonds with their male kin, as is seen in hamadryas ba­boons. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Pusey and Packer, 1987)

An in­ter­est­ing phe­nom­e­non in P. anu­bis is the sec­ondary trans­fer of aged adult males from their troops. Be­cause the abil­ity of a male to com­pete for mates is re­lated to youth and vigor, or to long-term so­cial re­la­tion­ships with fe­males, trans­fer to a new troop in old age can only re­duce a male’s op­por­tu­ni­ties to mate. In ad­di­tion, trans­fer­ring to a new group ex­poses a male to a great num­ber of haz­ards, in­clud­ing in­creased risk of pre­da­tion, and dan­gers from ag­gres­sion while in­te­grat­ing into the male dom­i­nance hi­er­ar­chy of the new group. How­ever, it ap­pears that aged males who once had high dom­i­nance ranks are the sub­jects of con­stant ha­rass­ment by younger males, who seem to re­mem­ber the for­mer "great­ness" of such older males. It seems that males with larger num­bers of fe­male "friends" are more likely to stay in their troop in spite of har­rass­ment. Males with­out many fe­male friends are more likely to trans­fer to new troops, thereby avoid­ing the costs of such ha­rass­ment. (Sapol­sky, 1996)

Be­cause males do not main­tain life-long so­cial ties with their kin, it is fe­male kin­ship that forms the core and sta­bil­ity of P. anu­bis so­ci­ety. Be­cause fe­males of this species do not em­i­grate from their natal groups, fe­male kin have life-long as­so­ci­a­tions. Within a troop of anu­bis ba­boons, there is a dom­i­nance hi­er­ar­chy of ma­tri­lines which is very sta­ble over time. In gen­eral, an in­di­vid­ual fe­male oc­cu­pies a place in the dom­i­nance hi­er­ar­chy im­me­di­ately below her mother and her younger sis­ters. Dom­i­nance re­la­tion­ships ap­pear to de­velop from in­fancy, when ma­ter­nal kin in­ter­vene in en­coun­ters with other ba­boons, and through the dif­fer­en­tial treat­ment of the young of higher-rank­ing fe­males by un­re­lated an­i­mals. Within a ma­tri­line, the dom­i­nance re­la­tion­ships of sis­ters are the in­verse of birth order. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987)

Fe­male kin be­have dif­fer­ently to­ward one an­other than do un­re­lated an­i­mals. Fe­males are more likely to aid their kin dur­ing dis­putes than they are to be­come in­volved in dis­putes of un­re­lated in­di­vid­u­als. These re­la­tion­ship can ex­tend over mul­ti­ple gen­er­a­tions, with fe­males aid­ing not only their own off­spring, and aunts aid­ing ne­ices, but grand­moth­ers aid­ing their grand­daugh­ters. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987)

The fe­male dom­i­nance hi­er­ar­chy is not con­tin­u­ally chal­lenged by the ar­rival of new fe­males in the troop, which prob­a­bly al­lows for the sta­bil­ity of fe­male dom­i­nance re­la­tion­ships over time. Per­haps be­cause re­lat­ed­ness of fe­males within troops is in­her­ently higher than the re­lat­ed­ness of fe­males be­tween troops, fe­male ba­boons are likely to take the lead in ag­gres­sive en­coun­ters with other troops of ba­boons. Fe­males are ap­par­ently act­ing to de­fend their food re­sources in such en­coun­ters. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987)

Within so­cially sim­i­lar cer­co­p­ithecines, fe­males have been known to es­tab­lish re­la­tion­ships across ma­tri­lines. Fe­males may be­friend oth­ers who are of sim­i­lar rank. Also, low rank­ing fe­males may at­tempt to gain favor with higher rank­ing fe­males by pro­vid­ing groom­ing and other af­fil­i­ate be­hav­iors. An ally with higher rank may pro­vide a low rank­ing fe­male with pref­er­en­tial ac­cess to scarce food re­sources, or sup­port dur­ing some ag­gres­sive en­coun­ters. (Bent­ley-Con­dit and Smith, 1999; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Smuts, 1987b)

Be­cause the dom­i­nance po­si­tion of ba­boons is so im­por­tant in their so­ci­ety, a great deal of in­ves­ti­ga­tion of po­ten­tial phys­i­o­log­i­cal causes of dom­i­nance has been con­ducted. Most in­ves­ti­gated have been lev­els of those hor­mones thought to be im­por­tant in ag­gres­sive be­hav­ior and stress re­sponse. Some in­ter­est­ing cor­re­la­tions be­tween en­docrine pro­files and be­hav­ioral pat­terns have emerged. (Sapol­sky and Ray, 1989; Vir­gin, Jr. and Sapol­sky, 1997; Sapol­sky and Ray, 1989; Vir­gin, Jr. and Sapol­sky, 1997)

In gen­eral, sub­or­di­nate males who ini­ti­ated fights with other males had higher testos­terone lev­els than males who did not ini­ti­ate fights. They also had lower base lev­els of glu­co­cor­ti­coids (hor­mones as­so­ci­ated with stress and stim­u­la­tion of the adrenal glands). When sub­or­di­nate males were in­volved fre­quently in con­sortships, they were more likely to move into the top half of the male dom­i­nance hi­er­ar­chy within 3 years than were males with­out fre­quent con­sortships. Like dom­i­nant males, they had re­duced basal glu­co­cor­ti­coid lev­els and a larger glu­co­cor­ti­coid re­sponse to stres­sors. (Sapol­sky and Ray, 1989; Vir­gin, Jr. and Sapol­sky, 1997)

Major be­hav­ioral traits that seemed to con­tribute to low­ered lev­els of glu­co­cor­ti­coid hor­mones were as­so­ci­ated with the so­cial "skill­ful­ness" of the an­i­mals. Males with low basal lev­els of glu­co­cor­ti­coids were bet­ter able to dis­tin­guish be­tween neu­tral and threat­en­ing in­ter­ac­tions with their ri­vals. They also were more likely to ini­ti­ate ag­gres­sion against ri­vals in threat­en­ing in­ter­ac­tions than were males with higher basal glu­co­cor­ti­coid lev­els. The males with lower glu­co­cor­ti­coid lev­els showed the great­est skill in de­ter­min­ing whether they had won or lost an ag­gres­sive en­counter, and also showed the great­est in­ci­dence of "dis­place­ment ag­gres­sion" (ag­gres­sion at some­thing other than the rival or sit­u­a­tion that was re­ally up­set­ting them). (Sapol­sky and Ray, 1989; Vir­gin, Jr. and Sapol­sky, 1997)

Al­though it is not cur­rently known whether the glu­co­cor­ti­coid lev­els of in­di­vid­u­als lead to the be­hav­iors, or visa versa, clearly, there are mea­sur­able dif­fer­ences be­tween in­di­vid­u­als which may allow pre­dic­tion of their fu­ture re­pro­duc­tive suc­cess. Fur­ther re­search in this area promises to help us to bet­ter un­der­stand the ge­netic and phys­i­o­log­i­cal un­der­pin­nings of so­cial be­hav­ior and suc­cess in this species. (Sapol­sky and Ray, 1989; Vir­gin, Jr. and Sapol­sky, 1997)

  • Range territory size
    0.39 to 1.968 km^2

Home Range

Home ranges be­tween 390 and 1,968 ha have been re­ported. The daily range of a troop av­er­ages 5,800 m. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987)

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

As in all highly so­cial species, com­mu­ni­ca­tion is var­ied and com­plex. Anu­bis ba­boons uti­lize vi­sual sig­nals and ges­tures, vo­cal­iza­tions, and tac­tile com­mu­ni­ca­tion. (Pri­mate Info Net, 2000)

Vi­sual sig­nals in­clude so­cial pre­sent­ing, in which a fe­male or ju­ve­nile dis­plays its hind quar­ters to a male. It can also be done by a fe­male who has ap­proached an­other fe­male with her black in­fant. This sub­mis­sive sig­nal dif­fers from sex­ual pre­sent­ing (which fe­males do to elicit cop­u­la­tion), and is often ac­com­pa­nied by lip smack­ing. Star­ing is a threat be­hav­ior, the ef­fect of which is en­hanced by the dif­fer­ently col­ored fur in the re­gion of the eye which is re­vealed when the ba­boon stares. Ca­nine tooth dis­play through a ten­sion yawn is an­other threat­en­ing ges­ture. It is per­formed by lower-rak­ing males to­ward higher-rank­ing males who are con­sort­ing with es­trus fe­males or who pos­sess meat. Male ba­boons who are close to one an­other can use tooth grind­ing to threaten one an­other. Ba­boons re­treat­ing from high-ten­sion sit­u­a­tions use rapid glances to break ten­sion. Adult males who are guard­ing mates some­times sit with their erect penis in plain view. This pe­nile dis­play com­mu­ni­cates the male’s pres­ence to other males. (Pri­mate Info Net, 2000)

Teeth chat­ter­ing and lips­mack­ing, al­though not tech­ni­cally vo­cal­iza­tions, are au­di­tory cues of re­as­sur­ance, often per­formed by a dom­i­nant an­i­mal when an­other is pre­sent­ing to it. (Pri­mate Info Net, 2000)

Vo­cal­iza­tions made by anu­bis ba­boons in­clude a two-phase bark, or "wahoo" call, which adult males di­rect to­ward fe­line preda­tors or to­ward other males. It is thought to com­mu­ni­cate the pres­ence of the male and his arousal. Adult male anu­bis ba­boons make grunt­ing vo­cal­iza­tions as a threat, and are known to "roar" dur­ing fights. A grat­ing roar, which is a deep, res­onat­ing call, is given by a dom­i­nant male after a fight, and is some­times made by adult males when there is a night-time dis­tur­bance. Screech­ing is com­mon dur­ing ag­gres­sive en­coun­ters, and can be made by any age or sex class. Subadult and adult olive ba­boons pro­duce a yakking call when re­treat­ing from a threat­en­ing an­i­mal. This call is often ac­com­pa­nied by a gri­mace of fear. A shrill bark is pro­duced by all ex­cept adult males to in­di­cate alarm, es­pe­cially due to sud­den dis­tur­bances. Fi­nally, rhyth­mic grunt­ing may be pro­duced by all anu­bis ba­boons ex­cept in­fants when they wish to sig­nal re­as­sur­ance to an­other an­i­mal. (Pri­mate Info Net, 2000)

Ju­ve­niles and in­fants pro­duce some vo­cal­iza­tions unique to their age class. These in­clude click­ing, which is a chirp-like noise which is anal­o­gous to yakking of adults. They also pro­duce an ick-ooer sound which com­mu­ni­cates a low level of dis­tress. (Pri­mate Info Net, 2000)

Tac­tile com­mu­ni­ca­tion is com­mon in cer­co­p­ithecines. So­cial groom­ing is used to re­in­force so­cial bonds, as well as to re­move par­a­sites and de­bris from the fur. So­cial mount­ing is a re­as­sur­ance be­hav­ior. Anu­bis ba­boons also per­form a friendly nose-to-nose greet­ing. (Pri­mate Info Net, 2000)

Chem­i­cal com­mu­ni­ca­tion has also been re­ported for this species. Fe­male anu­bis ba­boons are known to pro­duce aliphatic acids when they are sex­u­ally re­cep­tive. These acids are thought to en­hance a fe­male’s sex­ual at­trac­tive­ness. (Hrdy and Whit­ten, 1987)

Food Habits

Anu­bis ba­boons are known to eat a wide va­ri­ety of foods. They con­sume fruits, tree gums, in­sects, eggs, seeds, flow­ers, grass, rhi­zomes, corms, roots, tu­bers and small ver­te­brates. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Pri­mate Info Net, 2000; Strum, 1991)

One feed­ing adap­ta­tion thought to be shared by all ba­boons is the abil­ity to sub­sist on a rel­a­tively low qual­ity diet. Ba­boons can sub­sist on grasses for ex­tended pe­ri­ods of time. This al­lows them to ex­ploit dry ter­res­trial habi­tats, like deserts, semi­deserts, steppes, and grass­lands. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Oates, 1987)

  • Animal Foods
  • birds
  • mammals
  • reptiles
  • eggs
  • carrion
  • insects
  • terrestrial non-insect arthropods
  • Plant Foods
  • leaves
  • roots and tubers
  • seeds, grains, and nuts
  • fruit

Pre­da­tion

Two preda­tor species are known for anu­bis ba­boons. Leop­ards and chim­panzees have been known to kill these an­i­mals. Anu­bis ba­boons have been re­ported to mob leop­ards, often with adult males lead­ing the at­tack. In the Gombe pre­serve in Tan­za­nia, an es­ti­mated one per­cent of the pop­u­la­tion falls vic­tim to preda­tors an­nu­ally. Of these, about 3/4 are in­fants, and 1/4 are ju­ve­niles. (Ch­eney and Wrang­ham, 1987; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Nowak, 1999)

Ecosys­tem Roles

Ba­boons likely play a role in aer­at­ing the soil through the dig­ging of corms, roots, and tu­bers. They also are likely to dis­perse seeds of the fruits and grains that they eat. Ba­boons pro­vide food for their preda­tors, and also exert some af­fect on pop­u­la­tions of small an­i­mals on which they feed. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Pri­mate Info Net, 2000)

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Pos­i­tive

Anu­bis ba­boons are used in med­ical and be­hav­ioral re­search. They are so­cially ac­tive an­i­mals which pro­vide en­ter­tain­ment for zoo vis­i­tors and eco­tourists. (Nowak, 1999)

  • Positive Impacts
  • ecotourism
  • research and education

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Neg­a­tive

Ba­boons are large an­i­mals, and not par­tic­u­larly timid of hu­mans. They are known to raid crops, and can at­tack and in­jure hu­mans if pro­voked. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Nowak, 1999; Strum, 1991)

  • Negative Impacts
  • injures humans
    • bites or stings
  • crop pest

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

Anu­bis ba­boons are not con­sid­ered threat­ened or en­dan­gered. The IUCN Redlist rates them "Lower Risk, Least Con­cern." Like most pri­mates, they are in­cluded in Ap­pen­dix II of the CITES, so in­ter­na­tional trade in the an­i­mals or their parts re­quires gov­ern­ment ap­proval.

Other Com­ments

Hy­bridiza­tion be­tween Papio hamadryas and P. anu­bis oc­curs along the Awash river val­ley in Ethiopia. The area of hy­bridiza­tion ap­pears to be sta­ble, with­out no­tice­able in­tro­gres­sion of P. hamadryas phe­no­types into anu­bis ba­boon pop­u­la­tions or of P. anu­bis phe­no­types into hamadryas ba­boon pop­u­la­tions. The rea­sons for this sta­bil­ity are prob­a­bly very com­plex. How­ever, it is worth dis­cussing two con­trib­u­tors to this sta­bil­ity in this forum. (Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Nowak, 1999; Phillips-Con­roy, et al., 1992)

In hamadryas ba­boons, the basic so­cial unit, or OMU, is main­tained as a co­he­sive en­tity through the ac­tiv­ity of the adult male leader of the OMU. He herds fe­males and ju­ve­niles, reg­u­lates their in­ter­ac­tions, and pre­vents them from stray­ing. Al­though anu­bis ba­boon males pos­sess the same basic be­hav­iors that would allow the males of this species to form one-male-units, there are sig­nif­i­cant dif­fer­ences in ex­pres­sion be­tween the two species which make it im­pos­si­ble for male anu­bis ba­boons mi­grat­ing into hamadryas ter­ri­tory to suc­cess­fully main­tain a harem of fe­males. (Kum­mer, 1968; Phillips-Con­roy, et al., 1992)

For ex­am­ple, al­though male anu­bis ba­boons ag­gres­sively herd fe­males and ex­clude rival males, they tend to do so only when the fe­males are in es­trus. This would pre­vent a male anu­bis ba­boon from main­tain­ing a co­he­sive OMU in the hamadryas fash­ion. Also, al­though they form close so­cial ties with fe­males, they do not ex­hibit the "re­spect" of the re­la­tion­ship be­tween other males and their fe­males which is typ­i­cal of hamadryas ba­boons. This may be re­lated to dif­fer­ences in the kin as­so­ci­a­tions of hamadryas and anu­bis ba­boons. Cor­re­lated with this, an anu­bis male try­ing to "steal" a sex­u­ally at­trac­tive fe­male from a male hamadryas, would incur not just the wrath of that male, but likely the wrath of that male’s kin within the clan. (Kum­mer, 1968; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Nowak, 1999; Phillips-Con­roy, et al., 1992)

Hy­brid males are known to show be­hav­ior in­ter­me­di­ate be­tween the two parental species. Anu­bis-like hy­brids form last­ing so­cial bonds with ane­strous fe­males, and as­sume a con­sort-like sta­tus when the fe­males are in es­trus. How­ever, they are un­able to herd them ef­fi­ciently be­cause they do not ex­press this be­hav­ior when the fe­males are ane­strous. The more hamadryas-like hy­brids are ca­pa­ble of form­ing OMUS. (Phillips-Con­roy, et al., 1992)

In­ter­est­ingly, hamadryas males have been known to ef­fec­tively in­te­grate into anu­bis ba­boon troops. Al­though fe­males mate with them, these males may still be at a re­pro­duc­tive dis­ad­van­tage rel­a­tive to anu­bis males. Be­cause the mat­ing sys­tem of the hamadryas ba­boon char­ac­ter­is­ti­cally in­volves only one male, there has been lit­tle se­lec­tion for sperm com­pe­ti­tion in this species. Hamadryas males have both rel­a­tively and ab­solutely smaller tes­ti­cles than do anu­bis males. This likely re­sults in lower pro­duc­tion of sperm. Since fe­male anu­bis ba­boons may mate with a num­ber of males dur­ing their es­trus cycle, lower sperm pro­duc­tion by hamadryas males may lessen their chances for sir­ing off­spring. This may con­tribute to the sta­bil­ity of the hy­brid zone. (Jolly and Phillips-Con­roy, 2003; Mel­nick and Pearl, 1987; Phillips-Con­roy, et al., 1992)

Anu­bis ba­boons are also known to hy­bridize freely in the wild with yel­low ba­boons,. In the Am­boseli Na­tional Park in Kenya, the amount of re­ported hy­bridiza­tion be­tween these two species has in­creased over time. Re­searchers think that the in­creas­ing im­mi­gra­tion of anu­bis males into yel­low ba­boon troops is re­spon­si­ble for the in­crease noted in hy­brid char­ac­ters. Be­cause the slopes of Mount Kil­a­man­jaro are under in­creas­ing agri­cul­tural pres­sure, it is likely that anu­bis males have no al­ter­na­tive areas into which to em­i­grate. (Al­berts and Alt­mann, 2001; Jolly, 1993)

The only dif­fer­ences noted in be­hav­ior of hy­brid an­i­mals is that males with anu­bis-like fea­tures (e.g. coarser hair, longer manes, darker col­oration, broader chests, and more sharply "bro­ken" tails) tend to em­i­grate from their natal group as ju­ve­niles or subadults, rather than as full adults. This be­hav­ior has been seen oc­ca­sion­ally in the anu­bis ba­boons of the Gombe pre­serve in Tan­za­nia, but is not known in yel­low ba­boons. (Al­berts and Alt­mann, 2001; Pusey and Packer, 1987)

The hy­bridiza­tion be­tween anu­bis and yel­low ba­boons seems to have a long his­tory. In­ter­est­ing, the Ibean form of yel­low ba­boon, which has coarser hair than the typ­i­cal yel­low ba­boon, a more pro­nounced mane, and other some­what "anu­bis-like" fea­tures, is thought by some re­searchers to be ev­i­dence of the his­tor­i­cal in­flux of anu­bis genes into yel­low ba­boon pop­u­la­tions. (Al­berts and Alt­mann, 2001; Jolly, 1993)

Con­trib­u­tors

Nancy Shef­ferly (au­thor), An­i­mal Di­ver­sity Web, George Ham­mond (ed­i­tor), An­i­mal Di­ver­sity Web.

Glossary

Ethiopian

living in sub-Saharan Africa (south of 30 degrees north) and Madagascar.

World Map

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

agricultural

living in landscapes dominated by human agriculture.

altricial

young are born in a relatively underdeveloped state; they are unable to feed or care for themselves or locomote independently for a period of time after birth/hatching. In birds, naked and helpless after hatching.

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

carrion

flesh of dead animals.

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

desert or dunes

in deserts low (less than 30 cm per year) and unpredictable rainfall results in landscapes dominated by plants and animals adapted to aridity. Vegetation is typically sparse, though spectacular blooms may occur following rain. Deserts can be cold or warm and daily temperates typically fluctuate. In dune areas vegetation is also sparse and conditions are dry. This is because sand does not hold water well so little is available to plants. In dunes near seas and oceans this is compounded by the influence of salt in the air and soil. Salt limits the ability of plants to take up water through their roots.

diurnal
  1. active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
dominance hierarchies

ranking system or pecking order among members of a long-term social group, where dominance status affects access to resources or mates

ecotourism

humans benefit economically by promoting tourism that focuses on the appreciation of natural areas or animals. Ecotourism implies that there are existing programs that profit from the appreciation of natural areas or animals.

endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

fertilization

union of egg and spermatozoan

forest

forest biomes are dominated by trees, otherwise forest biomes can vary widely in amount of precipitation and seasonality.

iteroparous

offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

nomadic

generally wanders from place to place, usually within a well-defined range.

omnivore

an animal that mainly eats all kinds of things, including plants and animals

polygynandrous

the kind of polygamy in which a female pairs with several males, each of which also pairs with several different females.

rainforest

rainforests, both temperate and tropical, are dominated by trees often forming a closed canopy with little light reaching the ground. Epiphytes and climbing plants are also abundant. Precipitation is typically not limiting, but may be somewhat seasonal.

scrub forest

scrub forests develop in areas that experience dry seasons.

sedentary

remains in the same area

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

sexual ornamentation

one of the sexes (usually males) has special physical structures used in courting the other sex or fighting the same sex. For example: antlers, elongated tails, special spurs.

social

associates with others of its species; forms social groups.

soil aeration

digs and breaks up soil so air and water can get in

tactile

uses touch to communicate

terrestrial

Living on the ground.

territorial

defends an area within the home range, occupied by a single animals or group of animals of the same species and held through overt defense, display, or advertisement

tropical

the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.

tropical savanna and grassland

A terrestrial biome. Savannas are grasslands with scattered individual trees that do not form a closed canopy. Extensive savannas are found in parts of subtropical and tropical Africa and South America, and in Australia.

savanna

A grassland with scattered trees or scattered clumps of trees, a type of community intermediate between grassland and forest. See also Tropical savanna and grassland biome.

temperate grassland

A terrestrial biome found in temperate latitudes (>23.5° N or S latitude). Vegetation is made up mostly of grasses, the height and species diversity of which depend largely on the amount of moisture available. Fire and grazing are important in the long-term maintenance of grasslands.

visual

uses sight to communicate

viviparous

reproduction in which fertilization and development take place within the female body and the developing embryo derives nourishment from the female.

year-round breeding

breeding takes place throughout the year

Ref­er­ences

Al­berts, S., J. Alt­mann. 2001. Im­mi­gra­tion and hy­bridiza­tion pat­terns of yel­low and anu­bis ba­boons in and around Am­boseli, Kenya. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, `53: 139-154.

Alt­mann, J. 1980. Ba­boon Moth­ers and In­fants. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

Bent­ley-Con­dit, V., E. Smith. 1999. Fe­male dom­i­nance and fe­male so­cial re­la­tion­ships among yel­low ba­boons (Papio hamadryas cyno­cephalus). Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 47: 321-334.

Bercov­itch, F. 1987. Fe­male weight and re­pro­duc­tive con­di­tion in a pop­u­la­tion of olive ba­boons (Papio anu­bis). Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 12: 189-195.

Ch­eney, D., R. Wrang­ham. 1987. Pre­da­tion. Pp. 227-239 in B Smuts, D Ch­eney, R Sey­farth, R Wrang­hams, T Struh­saker, eds. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of chicago Press.

Groves, C. 2001. Pri­mate Tax­on­omy. Wash­ing­ton, D.C.: Smith­son­ian In­si­tu­tion Press.

Hamil­ton III, W., J. Bul­ger. 1992. Fac­ul­ta­tive ex­pre­sion of be­hav­ioral dif­fer­ences be­tween one-male and mul­ti­male sa­vanna ba­boon groups. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 28: 61-71.

Hrdy, S., P. Whit­ten. 1987. Pat­tern­ing of sex­ual ac­tiv­ity. Pp. 370-384 in B Smuts, D Ch­eney, R Sey­farth, R Wrang­ham, T Struh­saker, eds. Pri­mate So­cieities. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of Chicago PRess.

Jolly, C. 1993. Species, sub­species, and ba­boon sys­tem­at­ics. Pp. 67-107 in W Kim­bel, L Mar­tin, eds. Species, Species Con­cepts, and Pri­mate Evo­lu­tion. New York: Plenum Pub­lish­ing.

Jolly, C., J. Phillips-Con­roy. 2003. Tes­tic­u­lar size, mat­ing sys­tem, and mat­u­ra­tion sched­ules in wild anu­bis and hamadryas ba­boons. In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 24/1: 125-142.

Kum­mer, H. 1968. So­cial Or­gan­i­sa­tion of Hamdryas Ba­boons. A Field Study. Basel and Chicago: Karger, and Uni­ver­sity Press.

Mel­nick, D., M. Pearl. 1987. Cer­co­p­ithecines in mul­ti­male groups: Ge­netic di­ver­sity and pop­u­la­tion struc­ture. Pp. 121-134 in B Smuts, D Ch­eney, R Sey­farth, R Wrang­ham, T Struh­saker, eds. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

Napier, J., P. Napier. 1985. The nat­ural his­tory of the pri­mates. Cam­bridge, Mass­a­chu­setts: The MIT Press.

Nicol­son, N. 1987. In­fants, moth­ers, and other fe­males. Pp. 330-342 in B Smuts, D Ch­eney, R Sey­farth, R Wrang­ham, T Struh­saker, eds. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

Nowak, R. 1999. Walker's Mam­mals of the World, Sixth Edi­tion. Bal­ti­more: The Johns Hop­kins Uni­ver­sity Press.

Oates, J. 1987. Food dis­tri­b­u­tion and for­ag­ing be­hav­ior. Pp. 197-209 in B Smuts, D Ch­eney, R Sey­farth, R Wrang­ham, T Struh­saker, eds. Pri­mat So­ci­eties. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

Phillips-Con­roy, J., C. Jolly, P. Ny­s­trom, H. Hem­ma­lin. 1992. Mi­gra­tion of male hamadryas ba­boons into anu­bis groups in the Awash Na­tional Park, Ethiopia. In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 12/4: 455-475.

Pri­mate Info Net, 2000. "Pri­mate Info Net" (On-line). Olive Ba­boon (Papio anu­bis). Ac­cessed July 14, 2003 at http://​www.​primate.​wisc.​edu/​pin/​factsheets/​papio_​anubis.​html.

Pusey, A., C. Packer. 1987. Dis­per­sal and philopa­try. Pp. 250-266 in B Smuts, D Ch­eney, R Sey­farth, R Wrang­ham, T Struh­saker, eds. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

Rhine, R., R. Tilson. 1987. Re­ac­tions to fear as a prox­i­mate fac­tor in the so­ciospa­tial or­ga­ni­za­tion of ba­boon pro­gres­sions. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 13: 119-128.

Rogers, J. 2000. Mol­e­c­u­lar ge­netic vari­a­tion and pop­u­la­tion struc­ture in Papio ba­boons. Pp. 57-76 in P White­head, C Jolly, eds. Old World Mon­keys. Cam­bridge, UK: Cam­bridge Uni­ver­sity Press.

Sapol­sky, R., J. Ray. 1989. Styles of dom­i­nance and their en­docrine cor­re­lates among wild olive ba­boons (Papio anu­bis). Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 18: 1-13.

Sapol­sky, R. 1996. Why should an aged male ba­boon ever trans­fer troops?. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 39. Ac­cessed (Date Un­known) at 149-157.

Smuts, B. 1987. Gen­der, ag­gres­sion, and in­flu­ence. Pp. 400-412 in B Smuts, D Ch­eney, R Sey­farth, R Wrang­ham, T Struh­saker, eds. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

Smuts, B. 1987. Sex­ual com­pe­ti­tion and mate choice. Pp. 385-399 in B Smuts, D Ch­eney, R Sey­farth, R Wrang­ham, T Struh­saker, eds. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

Smuts, B., N. Nicol­son. 1989. Re­pro­duc­tion in wild fe­male olive ba­boons. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 19: 229-246.

Stein, D. 1984. The So­cio­bi­ol­ogy of Adult Male and In­fant Ba­boons. Nor­wood, NJ: Ablex Pub­lish­ing.

Strum, S. 1991. Weight and age in wild olive ba­boons. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 25: 219-237.

Vir­gin, Jr., C., R. Sapol­sky. 1997. Styles of male so­cial be­hav­ior and their en­docrine cor­re­lates among low-rank­ing male ba­boons. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 42: 25-39.

Wal­ters, J. 1987. Tran­si­tion to Adult­hood. Pp. 358-369 in B Smuts, D Ch­eney, R Sey­farth, R Wrang­ham, T Struh­saker, eds. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

Wal­ters, R., R. Sey­farth. 1987. Con­flict and co­op­er­a­tion. Pp. 306-317 in B Smuts, D Ch­eney, R Sey­farth, R Wrang­ham, T Struh­saker, eds. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

Whit­ten, P. 1987. In­fants and adult males. Pp. 343-357 in B Smuts, D Ch­eney, R Sey­farth, R Wrang­ham, T Struh­saker, eds. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

Williams-Blangero, S., J. Van­den­berg, J. Blangero, L. Konigs­berg, B. Dyke. 1990. Ge­netic dif­fer­en­ti­a­tion be­tween ba­boon sub­species: Rel­e­vance for bio­med­ical re­search. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 20: 67-81.