Trichosurus cunninghamimountain brushtail possum

Ge­o­graphic Range

Moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums, or south­ern bobucks, are na­tive to south­east­ern Aus­tralia, rang­ing from Vic­to­ria to Cen­tral Queens­land. (Lin­den­mayer, et al., 2002)

Habi­tat

Moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums are semi-ar­bo­real mar­su­pi­als found in wet scle­ro­phyll forests of south­east­ern Aus­tralia (Lin­den­mayer, Dubach, and Vig­gers, 2002). They usu­ally live in above-ground dens (ei­ther tree hol­lows or nest boxes). A re­cent study by J.K. Mar­tin found that adult bobucks use mul­ti­ple den-trees. How­ever, they also oc­ca­sion­ally den in thick ground veg­e­ta­tion (Mar­tin, 2006). Due to log­ging, farm­ing, and other human ac­tiv­i­ties, the pri­mary habi­tat of most pop­u­la­tions is now frag­mented for­est in agri­cul­tural land (Mar­tin and Han­dasyde, 2007). (Lin­den­mayer, et al., 2002; Mar­tin and Han­dasyde, 2007; Mar­tin, 2006)

  • Range elevation
    0 to 1300 m
    0.00 to 4265.09 ft
  • Average elevation
    300-1300 m
    ft

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

Moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums are medium-sized (2.6 to 4.2 kg) mar­su­pi­als. They have thick, light gray-brown fur, and long, dark gray, bushy tails. They dif­fer mor­pho­log­i­cally from their close rel­a­tives, short-eared pos­sums (Tri­chosu­rus can­i­nus), which is found di­rectly to the north of moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums. No sex­ual di­mor­phism is ap­par­ent (Lin­den­mayer, Dubach, and Vig­gers, 2002). (Lin­den­mayer, et al., 2002)

  • Sexual Dimorphism
  • sexes alike
  • Range mass
    2.5 to 4.5 kg
    5.51 to 9.91 lb
  • Range length
    81 to 93 cm
    31.89 to 36.61 in

Re­pro­duc­tion

Moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums ap­pear to be so­cially monog­a­mous. Adults are strongly paired and re­main very close to each other, stay­ing in the same den on ap­prox­i­mately 70% of the days dur­ing mat­ing sea­son. Fe­males form a pair-bond at 2 to 5 years of age and the bonds only end at the death of one pair mem­ber. How­ever, ge­netic ev­i­dence sug­gests that moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums are not monog­a­mous; mol­e­c­u­lar pa­ter­nity analy­sis has shown that 35% of young re­sult from ex­tra-pair cop­u­la­tions. Males that sired more than one off­spring a year, out­side of the pair bond, are gen­er­ally larger, in­di­cat­ing that dom­i­nance re­sults in re­pro­duc­tive suc­cess. Moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums have a short, syn­chro­nous breed­ing sea­son. Fe­males have one young per year. Young are in the pouch for ap­prox­i­mately 6 months, and then spends 1 to 2 months rid­ing on its mother’s back. Males play al­most no part in rais­ing the young (Mar­tin, Han­dasyde, Tay­lor, and Coul­son, 2007). (Mar­tin, et al., 2007)

Moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums breed once yearly, in Jan­u­ary and Feb­ru­ary. Off­spring re­main in the pouch for ap­prox­i­mately 6 months and then ride on their mother’s back for an­other 1 to 2 months. Off­spring are usu­ally weaned around Sep­tem­ber. Ju­ve­niles re­main close to their moth­ers until they are ap­prox­i­mately 18 months old. Fe­males be­come sex­u­ally ma­ture be­tween 2 and 5 years of age; males be­tween 2 and 3 years (Mar­tin, Han­dasyde, Tay­lor, and Coul­son, 2007). (Mar­tin, et al., 2007)

  • Breeding interval
    Mountain brushtail possums breed once yearly.
  • Breeding season
    The breeding season is January to February.
  • Range number of offspring
    0 to 1
  • Average number of offspring
    1
  • Range gestation period
    15 to 17 days
  • Range weaning age
    5 to 6 months
  • Range time to independence
    17 to 18 months
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    2 to 5 years
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    2 to 3 years
  • Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    3 years

Most moun­tain brush­tail pos­sum fe­males give birth to one baby a year. The off­spring spend ap­prox­i­mately 6 months in the pouch, then 1 to 2 months rid­ing on the mother’s back. Males spend the least amount of time with their fe­male part­ners when the fe­males are car­ry­ing young on their back, and are there­fore much less in­volved in the care of off­spring. Ju­ve­niles re­main with their moth­ers until they are ap­prox­i­mately 18 months old. (Mar­tin, et al., 2007)

  • Parental Investment
  • female parental care
  • pre-fertilization
    • provisioning
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-hatching/birth
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • protecting
      • female
  • post-independence association with parents
  • extended period of juvenile learning

Lifes­pan/Longevity

Both male and fe­male moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums ap­pear to be long-lived, reach­ing at least 12 years of age. How­ever, it ap­pears as though fewer males reach that age than fe­males (Mar­tin and Han­dasyde, 2007). They are per­haps the longest lived mar­su­pial (Vig­gers and Lin­den­mayer, 2002). (Mar­tin and Han­dasyde, 2007; Vig­gers and Lin­den­mayer, 2002)

  • Range lifespan
    Status: wild
    12 (high) years

Be­hav­ior

Moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums are noc­tur­nal, stay­ing in dens dur­ing the day and leav­ing at night to for­age. They are seden­tary, often re­main­ing in the same small home range for their en­tire lives. Adults form strong pair-bonds and often share the same suite of dif­fer­ent dens with their off­spring. The home ranges of paired in­di­vid­u­als over­lap as well, as op­posed to non-paired in­di­vid­u­als, who re­main more ex­clu­sive in their home ranges. (Mar­tin, et al., 2007)

  • Range territory size
    0.056 to 0.064 km^2

Home Range

Both male and fe­male adult moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums have a mean home range size of 6.0 hectares, plus or minus 0.4 hectares. The home ranges of subadults are sig­nif­i­cantly smaller. How­ever, this data was gath­ered in a for­est set­ting. There­fore, the home ranges of those pop­u­la­tions liv­ing in scrub set­tings is un­stud­ied and un­known (Mar­tin, 2006). (Mar­tin, 2006)

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

Like other pos­sums, Tri­chosu­rus cun­ning­hami re­lies on sight, hear­ing, touch, smell, and taste. Its whiskers en­hance it's per­cep­tion. (Mar­tin, 2006)

Food Habits

Moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums are her­biv­o­rous and fru­giv­o­rous. They mainly eat aca­cia leaves, fungi, lichens, buds, fruit, and some­times bark. Aca­cia is an in­te­gral part of their sea­sonal diet, with dif­fer­ent species of the plant con­sumed at dif­fer­ent times of the year. (Irl­beck and Hume, 2003)

  • Plant Foods
  • leaves
  • wood, bark, or stems
  • fruit
  • bryophytes
  • lichens
  • Other Foods
  • fungus

Pre­da­tion

Moun­tain brush­tail pos­sum preda­tors have not been well-re­ported. They are preyed on by non-na­tive foxes (Vulpes vulpes) and may also be taken by large snakes or rap­tors. The main threat to pop­u­la­tions is human ac­tion – the clear­ance of land for forestry and agri­cul­ture. (Menkhorst, et al., 2011)

Ecosys­tem Roles

Moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums may in­flu­ence veg­e­ta­tion com­mu­nity struc­ture through their her­bivory. Al­ler­gic re­ac­tions to ec­topar­a­sites on the skin may be one of the main causes of the dis­ease 'rump­wear' in moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums (Huf­schmid, Han­dasyde, and Bev­eridge, 2010). (Huf­schmid, et al., 2010; Menkhorst, et al., 2011)

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Pos­i­tive

Tri­chosu­rus cun­ning­hami does not ap­pear to be of pos­i­tive eco­nomic im­por­tance to hu­mans.

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Neg­a­tive

Moun­tain brush­tail pos­sums are some­times de­scribed as a de­struc­tive pest in south­east­ern Aus­tralian pine plan­ta­tions (IUCN Red List). (Menkhorst, et al., 2011)

  • Negative Impacts
  • crop pest

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

The con­ser­va­tion sta­tus of Tri­chosu­rus cun­ning­hami is of least con­cern, ac­cord­ing to the IUCN, and pop­u­la­tions are con­sid­ered sta­ble. (Menkhorst, et al., 2011)

Other Com­ments

Tri­chosu­rus cun­ning­hami was not de­scribed as a sep­a­rate species from Tri­chosu­rus can­i­nus until 2002. (Vig­gers and Lin­den­mayer, 2002)

Con­trib­u­tors

Helen Mc­Creary (au­thor), Yale Uni­ver­sity, Eric Sar­gis (ed­i­tor), Yale Uni­ver­sity, Rachel Raci­cot (ed­i­tor), Yale Uni­ver­sity, Tanya Dewey (ed­i­tor), Uni­ver­sity of Michi­gan-Ann Arbor.

Glossary

Australian

Living in Australia, New Zealand, Tasmania, New Guinea and associated islands.

World Map

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

arboreal

Referring to an animal that lives in trees; tree-climbing.

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

female parental care

parental care is carried out by females

folivore

an animal that mainly eats leaves.

forest

forest biomes are dominated by trees, otherwise forest biomes can vary widely in amount of precipitation and seasonality.

herbivore

An animal that eats mainly plants or parts of plants.

iteroparous

offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).

monogamous

Having one mate at a time.

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

nocturnal

active during the night

polygynous

having more than one female as a mate at one time

scent marks

communicates by producing scents from special gland(s) and placing them on a surface whether others can smell or taste them

scrub forest

scrub forests develop in areas that experience dry seasons.

seasonal breeding

breeding is confined to a particular season

sedentary

remains in the same area

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

social

associates with others of its species; forms social groups.

tactile

uses touch to communicate

temperate

that region of the Earth between 23.5 degrees North and 60 degrees North (between the Tropic of Cancer and the Arctic Circle) and between 23.5 degrees South and 60 degrees South (between the Tropic of Capricorn and the Antarctic Circle).

terrestrial

Living on the ground.

visual

uses sight to communicate

viviparous

reproduction in which fertilization and development take place within the female body and the developing embryo derives nourishment from the female.

Ref­er­ences

Huf­schmid, J., K. Han­dasyde, I. Bev­eridge. 2010. The role of host and en­vi­ron­men­tal fac­tors in the epi­demi­ol­ogy of rump­wear in brush­tail pos­sums. Aus­tralian Jour­nal of Zo­ol­ogy, 58: 250-262. Ac­cessed May 09, 2012 at http://​www.​publish.​csiro.​au/?​act=view_​file&​file_​id=ZO10030.​pdf.

Irl­beck, N., I. Hume. 2003. The role of Aca­cia in the diets of Aus­tralian mar­su­pi­als – a re­view. Aus­tralian Mam­mal­ogy, 25: 121-134.

Lin­den­mayer, D., J. Dubach, K. Vig­gers. 2002. Ge­o­graphic di­mor­phism in the moun­tain brush­tail pos­sum T. can­i­nus: the case for a new species. Aus­tralian Jour­nal of Zo­ol­ogy, 50: 369-393.

Mar­tin, J. 2006. Den-use and home-range char­ac­ter­is­tics of bobucks, Tri­chosu­rus cun­ning­hami, res­i­dent in a for­est patch. Aus­tralian Jour­nal of Zo­ol­ogy, 54: 225-234.

Mar­tin, J., K. Han­dasyde. 2007. Com­par­i­son of Bobuck de­mog­ra­phy in two habi­tat types in the Strath­bo­gie Ranges, Aus­tralia. Jour­nal of Zo­ol­ogy, 271: 375-385.

Mar­tin, J., K. Han­dasyde, A. Tay­lor, G. Coul­son. 2007. Long-term pair-bonds with­out mat­ing fi­delity in a mam­mal. Be­hav­iour, 144: 1419-1445.

Menkhorst, P., D. Tag­gart, M. Ellis, R. Mar­tin. 2011. "The IUCN Red List of Threat­ened Species" (On-line). Ac­cessed April 10, 2012 at http://​www.​iucnredlist.​org/​apps/​redlist/​details/​136256/​0.

Vig­gers, K., D. Lin­den­mayer. 2002. The Other Brush­tail Pos­sum. Na­ture Aus­tralia, Spring 27: 46.