Presbytis thomasinorth Sumatran leaf monkey

Ge­o­graphic Range

Pres­bytis thomasi is one of sev­eral lan­gurs found in the Ori­en­tal is­lands. How­ever, its ge­o­graphic range is fairly lim­ited. Its na­tive home con­sists of North Suma­tra of In­done­sia—more specif­i­cally, north of Sun­gai Wampu and Sun­gai Sim­pang Kiri. (Col­ijn and Muchtar, 1996; Nowak, 1999)

Habi­tat

When search­ing through trop­i­cal rain­forests, rub­ber plan­ta­tions, and both pri­mary and sec­ondary forests, one is able to en­counter Thomas’s lan­gur. Since it is ar­bo­real, one usu­ally needs to search high in the trees to find this unique species. How­ever, its po­si­tion­ing in the trees de­pends on what time of day it is. While it is tak­ing a nap dur­ing the day, it se­lects a tree that tends to have lots of twigs and leaves for pro­tec­tion from the harsh sun­light. How­ever, when it is sleep­ing at night, it sleeps in the top of a tall tree that faces the open areas. (Col­ijn and Muchtar, 1996; Flan­nery, 2004; Gur­maya, 1986)

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

Pres­bytis thomasi has a very dis­tinct ap­pear­ance. Due to their unique fa­cial col­oration, it is easy to dis­tin­guish North Suma­tran leaf mon­keys from other pri­mates. The white fur on the un­der­side and arms (which con­trasts with the black fur sur­round­ing the rest of the body) con­tin­ues up around the neck. Two other white stripes, start­ing from the top of the head, run down the sides, come to­gether in a V-shape at the eyes and en­cir­cle them. A pur­ple-sil­ver col­ored inner layer forms rings around the or­ange-brown eyes. In­side of the dark tint, one can see the pink­ish skin of Thomas's lan­gur. This same pink­ish skin cov­ers the muz­zle.

The av­er­age size of Thomas's lan­gur is 6.69 kilo­grams for adult fe­males and 6.67 kilo­grams for adult males. The tail length is be­tween 500 and 850 mm, and the head and body length ranges be­tween a mere 420 and 620 mm. (Col­ijn and Muchtar, 1996; Flan­nery, 2004; Nowak, 1999)

  • Sexual Dimorphism
  • sexes alike
  • Range mass
    5 to 8.1 kg
    11.01 to 17.84 lb
  • Range length
    92 to 147 cm
    36.22 to 57.87 in

Re­pro­duc­tion

The mat­ing sys­tem of P. thomasi is de­bated. In the En­cy­clo­pe­dia of Mam­mals, it is noted that the species is a monog­a­mous pri­mate. The fe­male ini­ti­ates the mat­ing by per­form­ing var­i­ous acts to per­suade her male coun­ter­part, such as re­leas­ing cer­tain smells and dis­play­ing gen­i­talia. ("Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys", 2001)

How­ever, oth­ers dis­pute the monogamy of the species. Steen­beck, et al. (1999) states that within groups, there are often sev­eral fe­males and one breed­ing male. A pos­si­ble res­o­lu­tion be­tween the two ob­ser­va­tions is that only one of the fe­males in the group breeds with the male while the other fe­males help raise the young. (Steen­beek, et al., 1999)

There is no spec­i­fied breed­ing in­ter­val for Thomas’s lan­gurs. How­ever, even though the breed­ing sea­son is not re­stricted, there tends to be an in­crease in mat­ing when the fol­low­ing wean­ing pe­riod is ex­pected to cor­re­spond with an abun­dance of food. The ges­ta­tion pe­riod lasts 5 to 6 months. At the end of ges­ta­tion, fe­male P. thomasi pro­duce one young one young at a time. Fe­males rarely pro­duce more than one off­s­r­p­ing at a time, and never more than two. Wean­ing oc­curs at 12 to 15 months, after which an off­spring is sup­posed to be­come in­de­pen­dent. Al­though they are fully in­de­pen­dent, ju­ve­niles do not reach sex­ual ma­tu­rity until 4 or 5 years of age. ("Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys", 2001; Eimerl and De­Vore, 1965)

  • Breeding interval
    Breeding can occur every 1.5 to 2 years.
  • Breeding season
    The breeding season of these monkeys is not restricted.
  • Range number of offspring
    1 (low)
  • Average number of offspring
    1
  • Range gestation period
    5 to 6 months
  • Range weaning age
    12 to 15 months
  • Range time to independence
    15 to 18 months
  • Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    4 years
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    4 to 5 years

When car­ing for her young, the mother re­moves her­self from the dom­i­nance struc­ture. Other fe­males of the group are often at­tracted to the young due to the dis­tinct col­or­ing, and so they care for and pro­tect the young when­ever pos­si­ble. As soon as the in­fant be­comes upset or dis­traught, an­other fe­male quickly tries to com­fort it. The male in­fant has no con­tact with a male adult until he is 10 months old. Dur­ing pre-wean­ing, the young has to learn what to eat, what to avoid, and other be­hav­ioral tac­tics in order to sur­vive. ("Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys", 2001; Eimerl and De­Vore, 1965)

The male in­fant has no con­tact with a male adult until he is 10 months old. A fe­male in­fant, how­ever, has no con­tact with an adult male until she is 3.5 to 4 years old. Often dur­ing wean­ing, a male within the group or an out­side group com­mits in­fan­ti­cide, killing an in­fant so that the mother can re­gain her nor­mal cycle of fer­til­ity faster than she would if her child were still alive. This may ex­plain the delay in con­tact be­tween young and adult males. ("Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys", 2001; Eimerl and De­Vore, 1965)

  • Parental Investment
  • precocial
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • extended period of juvenile learning

Lifes­pan/Longevity

The av­er­age lifes­pan of P. thomasi is 20 years. In cap­tiv­ity, the av­er­age lifes­pan is 29 years. The nine-year dif­fer­ence may be due to nu­mer­ous fac­tors such as the de­struc­tion of habi­tat, hunt­ing by hu­mans, the pres­ence of nat­ural preda­tors, and at­tacks be­tween neigh­bor­ing groups. ("Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys", 2001)

  • Average lifespan
    Status: wild
    20 years
  • Average lifespan
    Status: captivity
    29 years

Be­hav­ior

These lan­gurs spend most of the day in groups ei­ther rest­ing, feed­ing, or mov­ing. These groups usu­ally con­sist of five fe­males and one male, but there can also be small groups of males or in­di­vid­ual males liv­ing alone.

Within these groups, there is a dom­i­nance hi­er­ar­chy that every­one, both male and fe­male, is sub­jected to. It has been sug­gested that an in­di­vid­ual’s rank within the hi­er­ar­chy may de­pend upon its age or its abil­ity to de­fend it­self against oth­ers in the group. ("Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys", 2001; Eimerl and De­Vore, 1965; Gur­maya, 1986; Steen­beek, et al., 1999)

Even though this lan­gur is known for hav­ing a calmer de­meanor in ges­tures and re­sponses to con­specifics than other pri­mates, com­pe­ti­tion has been ob­served within groups. Com­pe­ti­tion in­creases with the size of the group, and fem­laes show a pref­er­ence for smaller groups due to the de­crease in risk of in­fan­ti­cide. Group sizes are often de­pen­dent upon fe­male dis­per­sal. ("Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys", 2001; Steen­beck and van Schaik, 2001)

Many fac­tors im­pact a fe­male’s de­ci­sion to leave her group and join an­other. These in­clude com­pe­ti­tion for food, the risk of pre­da­tion, and avoid­ance of in­breed­ing. More im­por­tant is the avoid­ance of in­fan­ti­cide. Often a fe­male leaves her group to pro­tect her young from in­fan­ti­cide. (Sterck, 1997)

In­fan­ti­cide ap­pears to be a very im­por­tant be­hav­ior for P. thomasi. In­fan­ti­cide usu­ally oc­curs when males at­tack. Fe­males do not typ­i­cally at­tack un­less de­fend­ing their young from out­side males. Fe­males are much more ag­gres­sive when they have an in­fant than they are when they have no de­pen­dent off­spring. When the group male is within 5 me­ters of a mother and her child, she is sig­nif­i­cantly less vig­i­lant, ap­par­ently be­cause the pres­ence of the male so close by is an as­sur­ance of pro­tec­tion. (Steen­beek, et al., 1999)

  • Range territory size
    500 to 800 m^2

Home Range

Like most lan­gurs, P. thomasi is both di­ur­nal and ar­bo­real. It oc­cas­sion­aly re­trieves min­eral re­sources from the ground. Groups re­lo­cate to dif­fer­ent areas within the forests by mi­grat­ing through the trees. This so­cial species is also very ter­ri­to­r­ial, often de­fend­ing its area by bark­ing or even at­tack­ing out­siders. (Nowak, 1999)

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

Vocal com­mu­ni­ca­tion is at its most in­tense and fre­quent at dawn. It is uti­lized in a va­ri­ety of sit­u­a­tions such as re­lo­ca­tion, at­tack­ing, es­tab­lish­ing sleep­ing po­si­tions, de­fend­ing ter­ri­tory and mates. Vo­cal­iza­tions are ac­com­pa­nied by ol­fac­tory com­mu­ni­ca­tion when mat­ing is in­ti­ti­ated. ("Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys", 2001; Eimerl and De­Vore, 1965)

Thomas' lan­gurs use nu­mer­ous types of vocal calls. For ex­am­ple, an alpha male tends to make a se­ries of “choom” sounds when he is ex­cited; how­ever, when he is in­volved in an in­ter-troop or group-troop en­counter, he makes a se­ries of “kak” and “ngkung” sounds. Sim­i­larly, when threat­ened, ju­ve­niles make an al­ter­nat­ing se­ries of “kek”s and “wek”s. Ag­gres­sive fe­males make “kuk” sounds. (Gur­maya, 1986)

Vocal and vi­sual com­mu­ni­ca­tion de­velop as these mon­keys ma­ture. In in­fant P. thomasi, com­mu­ni­ca­tion is re­stricted to whin­ing and squeal­ing. Once an in­di­vid­ual be­comes a ju­ve­nile, its abil­i­ties have broad­ened to scream­ing, gri­mac­ing, slap­ping the ground, pre­sent, alarm bark­ing, star­ing, and threat bob­bing. Fi­nally, as an adult, it no longer squeals or screams, but barks and par­takes in dom­i­nance fight­ing. ("Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys", 2001; Eimerl and De­Vore, 1965)

As in all pri­mates, tac­tile com­mu­ni­ca­tion, be­tween mates, ri­vals, as well as be­tween moth­ers and their off­spring is im­por­tant. Tac­tile com­mu­ni­ca­tion in­cludes groom­ing, re­as­sur­ance, and ag­gres­sion.

Food Habits

This leaf-mon­key’s diet is pri­mar­ily cen­tered around fruits, leaves, and seeds. How­ever, it will eat other things such as flow­ers, bark, twigs, stalks of co­conuts, leaf stalks, birds, bird eggs, algae, and some in­sects. Water is made avail­able in tree holes. Oc­ca­sional vis­its to the ground are made in order to ob­tain ants, toad­stools, soil, and snails. Large peaks in for­ag­ing occur three times per day, ac­com­pa­nied by rest­ing in the lower por­tions of trees. For­ag­ing be­hav­ior is highly in­flu­enced by the risk of pre­da­tion. (Flan­nery, 2004; Gur­maya, 1986; Nowak, 1999; Sterck, 2002; Swindler, 1998)

  • Animal Foods
  • birds
  • eggs
  • insects
  • mollusks
  • Plant Foods
  • leaves
  • wood, bark, or stems
  • seeds, grains, and nuts
  • fruit
  • flowers
  • algae
  • Other Foods
  • fungus

Pre­da­tion

A char­ac­ter­is­tic shared among most of P. thomasi's preda­tors is the abil­ity to climb trees. Preda­tors such as retic­u­lated pythons, clouded leop­ards, tigers, and golden cats are often suc­cess­ful in cap­tur­ing Thomas’s lan­gurs. Be­cause these lan­gurs are able to move swiftly through branches, their preda­tors have been found to at­tack only when the dis­tance be­tween them and their prey is very short. Preda­tors are more ef­fec­tive when at­tack­ing P. thomasi on the ground rather than in trees. Be­cause of this, the most dan­ger­ous area for these mon­keys is in the lower strata of the for­est, which is 0 to 10m.

Thomas' lan­gurs rely on two valu­able forms of pro­tec­tion: their ar­bo­real habits and the alarm calls that are pro­duced when groups are around. Thomas’s lan­gurs climb down from trees more often when they are sur­rounded by neigh­bors.

Fe­males are more prone to pre­da­tion due to their pref­er­ence for snails and their smaller ca­nines. Males are often found nearby in trees look­ing out for preda­tors when fe­males ven­ture down to the ground by them­selves. (Flan­nery, 2004; Sterck, 2002)

Ecosys­tem Roles

Be­cause of its pref­er­ence for fruits, flow­ers, and seeds, one can infer that P. thomasi may dis­perse seeds and help pol­li­nate plants while feed­ing. Like many pri­mates, P. thomasi may also be a host to par­a­sites such as fleas and ticks. To the ex­tent that these mon­keys serve as prey for other an­i­mals, they may im­pact preda­tor pop­u­la­tions. (Flan­nery, 2004)

  • Ecosystem Impact
  • disperses seeds
  • pollinates

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Pos­i­tive

These lan­gurs are not known to have any par­tic­u­lar ben­e­fit to hu­mans.

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Neg­a­tive

Due to their pri­mary diet of fruits, leaves, and seeds, Thomas's lan­gurs are known to dis­rupt the crops of neigh­bor­ing hu­mans. ("Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys", 2001)

  • Negative Impacts
  • crop pest

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

Due to an in­crease in the avail­abil­ity of firearms and the de­struc­tion of forests in Suma­tra, P. thomasi has to face los­ing its habi­tat and being hunted by neigh­bor­ing hu­mans. An­other fac­tor which may be re­spon­si­ble for de­clin­ing pop­u­la­tions is ha­bit­ual in­fan­ti­cide that per­sists in this species. This be­hav­ior has been known to have in­creased in re­cent years; how­ever, an in­crease in aware­ness has in­di­cated that this be­hav­ior is very im­por­tant to the species. ("Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys", 2001; "Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys", 2001; Steen­beek, et al., 1999)

Con­trib­u­tors

Nancy Shef­ferly (ed­i­tor), An­i­mal Di­ver­sity Web.

Matthew Wund (ed­i­tor), Uni­ver­sity of Michi­gan-Ann Arbor.

Mika Matthews (au­thor), Uni­ver­sity of Michi­gan-Ann Arbor, Phil Myers (ed­i­tor, in­struc­tor), Mu­seum of Zo­ol­ogy, Uni­ver­sity of Michi­gan-Ann Arbor.

Glossary

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

arboreal

Referring to an animal that lives in trees; tree-climbing.

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

choruses

to jointly display, usually with sounds, at the same time as two or more other individuals of the same or different species

cooperative breeder

helpers provide assistance in raising young that are not their own

diurnal
  1. active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
dominance hierarchies

ranking system or pecking order among members of a long-term social group, where dominance status affects access to resources or mates

endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

fertilization

union of egg and spermatozoan

folivore

an animal that mainly eats leaves.

forest

forest biomes are dominated by trees, otherwise forest biomes can vary widely in amount of precipitation and seasonality.

frugivore

an animal that mainly eats fruit

granivore

an animal that mainly eats seeds

herbivore

An animal that eats mainly plants or parts of plants.

island endemic

animals that live only on an island or set of islands.

iteroparous

offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).

monogamous

Having one mate at a time.

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

nomadic

generally wanders from place to place, usually within a well-defined range.

oriental

found in the oriental region of the world. In other words, India and southeast Asia.

World Map

pheromones

chemicals released into air or water that are detected by and responded to by other animals of the same species

rainforest

rainforests, both temperate and tropical, are dominated by trees often forming a closed canopy with little light reaching the ground. Epiphytes and climbing plants are also abundant. Precipitation is typically not limiting, but may be somewhat seasonal.

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

social

associates with others of its species; forms social groups.

tactile

uses touch to communicate

terrestrial

Living on the ground.

territorial

defends an area within the home range, occupied by a single animals or group of animals of the same species and held through overt defense, display, or advertisement

tropical

the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.

visual

uses sight to communicate

viviparous

reproduction in which fertilization and development take place within the female body and the developing embryo derives nourishment from the female.

year-round breeding

breeding takes place throughout the year

young precocial

young are relatively well-developed when born

Ref­er­ences

An­drom­eda Ox­ford Ltd. 2001. Colobus and Leaf Mon­keys. Pp. 380-393 in D Mac­don­ald, ed. The En­cy­clo­pe­dia of Mam­mals, Vol. II: Pri­mates and Large Her­bi­vores, 2nd Edi­tion. 132 West 31st Street, New York NY 10001: Facts on File, Inc..

Col­ijn, E., M. Muchtar. 1996. "Pri­mates of In­done­sia--Pres­bytis Thomasi (Col­let, 1893)" (On-line). The In­done­sian Na­ture Con­ser­va­tion Data­base. Ac­cessed Feb­ru­ary 08, 2004 at http://​www.​nature-conservation.​or.​id/​primates/​presbytis_​thomasi.​html.

Eimerl, S., I. De­Vore. 1965. Life Na­ture Li­brary: The Pri­mates. New York: Time-Life Books.

Flan­nery, S. 2004. "Thomas's Leaf-mon­key (Pres­bytis thomasi)" (On-line). The Pri­mata. Ac­cessed April 10, 2004 at http://​members.​tripod.​com/​uakari/​presbytis_​thomasi.​html.

Gur­maya, K. 1986. Ecol­ogy and Be­hav­ior of Pres­bytis thomasi in North­ern Suma­tra. Pri­mates, 27(2): 151-172.

Nowak, R. 1999. Pri­mates; Fam­ily CER­CO­P­ITHE­CI­DAE: Old World Mon­keys. Pp. 599-600 in Walker's Mam­mals of the World, Vol. I, 6th Edi­tion. Bal­ti­more and Lon­don: The Johns Hop­kins Uni­ver­sity Press.

Steen­beck, R., C. van Schaik. 2001. Com­pe­ti­tion and group sive in Thomas's lan­gurs (Pres­bytis thomasi): the fo­li­vore para­dox re­vis­ited. Be­hav­ioral Ecol­ogy and So­cio­bi­ol­ogy, 49(2-3): 100-110. Ac­cessed March 07, 2004 at http://​www.​springerlink.​com/​media/​gmuavwqywj238ypgdmwk/​Contributions/​X/​E/​T/​3/​XET3VU7B5K8Q97CN.​pdf.

Steen­beek, R., R. Piek, M. van Buul, J. van Hooff. 1999. Vig­i­lance in wild Thomas's lan­gurs (Pres­bytis thomasi): the im­por­tance of in­fan­ti­cide risk. Be­hav­ioral Ecol­ogy and So­cio­bi­ol­ogy, 45: 137-150. Ac­cessed Feb­ru­ary 11, 2004 at http://​www.​springerlink.​com/​media/​np330r1a8gdrrjcauq7p/​Contributions/​4/​R/​A/​U/​4RAUVQNAP5B6P0JR.​pdf.

Sterck, E. 1997. De­ter­mi­nants of fe­male dis­per­sal in Thomas Lan­gurs. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 42(3): 179-198.

Sterck, E. 2002. Preda­tor sen­si­tive for­ag­ing in Thomas lan­gurs. Pp. 74-91 in L Miller, ed. Eat or be eaten: Preda­tor sen­si­tive for­ag­ing among pri­mates. Cam­bridge, UK: Cam­bridge Uni­ver­sity Press.

Swindler, D. 1998. In­tro­duc­tion to the Pri­mates. Seat­tle and Lon­don: Uni­ver­sity of Wash­ing­ton Press.