Piliocolobus rufomitratusTana river red colobus

Ge­o­graphic Range

Pil­io­colobus ru­fomi­tra­tus is lim­ited to a small range of for­est along the lower Tana River in East­ern Kenya. The Tana River runs from the east­ern Kenyan High­lands into the In­dian Ocean. Its range in­cludes the Tana River Na­tional Pri­mate Re­serve out­side the city of Wenje. (Med­ley, 1993)

Habi­tat

Pil­io­colobus ru­fomi­tra­tus, or Tana River red colobus mon­key, in­hab­its patches of ever­green river­ine for­est that un­dergo pe­ri­odic flood­ing from the Tana River. These iso­lated clumps of for­est are under the in­flu­ence of ground­wa­ter fluc­tu­a­tion, river course changes and human dis­tur­bance which re­sult in a very frag­mented and un­sta­ble habi­tat only fur­ther en­dan­gered by human en­croach­ment (Decker & Kin­naird, 1992). This river­ine for­est habi­tat ex­pe­ri­ences an av­er­age an­nual pre­cip­i­ta­tion of 470 mm and tem­per­a­tures rang­ing be­tween 21.4°C and 33°C (Med­ley, 1993). In con­trast to the habi­tat of the other red colobus species across East and Cen­tral Africa, this habi­tat is more arid and has a lower den­sity and di­ver­sity of tree flora (Marsh, 1981a). In gen­eral, the canopy of the Tana River for­est is more open than the habi­tat at other red colobus sites (Oates, 1994). (Decker and Kin­naird, 1992; Marsh, 1981a; Med­ley, 1993; Oates, 1994)

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

The pelage of P. ru­fomi­tra­tus is ruddy black on the dor­sal side and red with brown on the chest, inner por­tions of the limbs and head (Nowak, 1999). Head and body length ranges be­tween 450 and 670 mm. The tail is gray and fairly long, mea­sur­ing 520 mm to 800 mm. This species ex­hibits sev­eral traits that may be adap­ta­tions to an ar­bo­real lifestyle, in­clud­ing long, slen­der dig­its and the loss of the thumb tu­ber­cle, com­mon in all colobine mon­keys. The hind limbs are well de­vel­oped for leap­ing across the canopy and may be par­tic­u­larly long. Though no de­fin­i­tive study on mass has been com­pleted, weights may range be­tween 5.1 and 11.3 kg like other red colobus species. A closer es­ti­mate may put their mean weight at 5.8 kg, near that of the more iso­lated West African species of the Pro­colobus kirki (Oates, 1994). (Nowak, 1999; Oates, 1994)

  • Sexual Dimorphism
  • sexes alike
  • Range mass
    5.1 to 11.3 kg
    11.23 to 24.89 lb
  • Average mass
    5.8 kg
    12.78 lb
  • Range length
    450 to 670 mm
    17.72 to 26.38 in

Re­pro­duc­tion

The mat­ing sys­tem in this species is gen­er­ally polyg­y­nous, es­pe­cially when only one male is pre­sent. Often the dom­i­nance hi­er­ar­chy de­ter­mines who is al­lowed mat­ing ac­cess, and in the pe­cu­liar case of Tana River red colobus mon­keys, there is fre­quently a sin­gle male or a small group of males that pro­hibits ex­tra-group males from mat­ing or even en­ter­ing the group. (New­ton and Dun­bar, 1994; Marsh, 1979) (Marsh, 1979; New­ton and Dun­bar, 1994)

Very lit­tle is known about the re­pro­duc­tive cycle of these pri­mates in the wild. They are dif­fi­cult to find and track due to their con­stant mi­gra­tion.

Un­like most colobine mon­keys, red colobus fe­males ex­hibit pe­ri­odic swelling of per­ineal tis­sue through­out the year (Nowak, 1999; Oates, 1994). These swellings often co­in­cide with an in­crease in cop­u­la­tion, so it is as­sumed that they mark the time of ovu­la­tion, though false swellings (those not cor­re­lated with ovu­la­tion) have been seen in other pri­mates and may serve as a pos­si­ble de­fense against in­fan­ti­cide. There ap­pears to be no dis­tinct birthing sea­son though stud­ies to de­ter­mine rel­a­tive birth spac­ing are lack­ing (Marsh, 1979).

Typ­i­cally the dom­i­nant male mates with the fe­males in his so­cial group. Cop­u­la­tions are ini­ti­ated by fe­males who per­form courtship dis­plays and pre­sent them­selves to the male. Be­fore mat­ing, there are fre­quent cop­u­la­tion calls serv­ing to draw males into the area. Dur­ing mat­ing, the fe­males may give other calls that con­tinue to at­tract males (Struh­saker, 1975; Oates, 1994). Ha­rass­ment dur­ing cop­u­la­tion by non-mat­ing males was fre­quent in the rain-for­est species (Struh­saker, 1975); how­ever, there are sig­nif­i­cantly fewer males in Tana River red colobus groups (often only one or two males) so the im­pact of this ha­rass­ment is prob­a­bly neg­li­gi­ble (Marsh, 1979). There has not been a de­fin­i­tive study on the im­pact of the smaller group size, lower male num­bers and re­duced habi­tat on the re­pro­duc­tion of Tana River red colobus mon­keys, and this could be an in­ter­est­ing re­search op­por­tu­nity. (Marsh, 1979; Nowak, 1999; Oates, 1994; Struh­saker, 1975)

In gen­eral, red colobus mon­keys breed through­out the year. Es­ti­mated ges­ta­tion is 4.5 to 5.5 months, with an es­ti­mated in­ter­birth in­ter­val of 26 months. The exact pe­riod of nurs­ing has not been re­ported for these an­i­mals, how­ever, we may infer that wean­ing prob­a­bly oc­curs be­fore the in­fant is 20 months old, as con­tin­ued nurs­ing might in­hibit con­cep­tion of an­other off­spring. (Nowak, 1999; Nowak, 1999)

  • Breeding interval
    Females are capable of producing an offspring roughly every 2 years.
  • Breeding season
    Breeding is not seasonal.
  • Average number of offspring
    1
  • Range gestation period
    4.5 to 5.5 months

At birth the in­fant’s pelage is silky black on the back with a gray un­der­belly, help­ing to dis­tin­guish in­fants from older mon­keys. Pink flesh is ex­posed on the muz­zles, ears, palms and soles for 3 to 4 weeks (Oates, 1994). The fur re­mains gray until 2 to 2.5 months of age, when the head be­gins to turn red­dish on the crown.

After birth, in­fants cling to their moth­ers' un­der­belly and re­main de­pen­dent on her. In gen­eral in­fants stay very close to the mother until 2 to 3.5 months of age, trav­el­ing only about 1 meter from her dur­ing this this time. By 3.5 to 5.5 months they typ­i­cally en­gage in play with other mon­keys (Marsh, 1979). NAround 18 months of age, young fe­males of the group em­i­grate to an­other group and may con­tinue to move from group to group through­out their life­time, as fe­male mem­ber­ship is very fluid. Some males may leave the group dur­ing ado­les­cence but it is dif­fi­cult to be­come ac­cepted into an­other group in this pa­tri­lin­eal so­cial struc­ture (Struh­saker, 1975).

Often only the mother grooms and car­ries the in­fant and in­fant han­dling, al­lo­moth­er­ing or aunt­ing (han­dling/care of the in­fants by non-moth­ers) are not com­mon be­hav­iors. This is most likely be­cause the pa­tri­lin­eal dis­tri­b­u­tion pat­tern in which fe­males em­i­grate from their natal group re­sults in low lev­els of re­lat­ed­ness be­tween fe­males in the same group. Un­re­lated fe­males are much less likely to allow their in­fants to be han­dled by other fe­males. This is un­like the pat­tern seen in ba­boons, in which the fe­males are more closely re­lated to one an­other (New­ton and Dun­bar, 1994).

Males con­tribute very lit­tle to the off­spring other than prox­i­mal pro­tec­tion from pre­da­tion and re­source al­lo­ca­tion. There doesn’t seem to be any re­la­tion­ship be­tween the dom­i­nance hi­er­ar­chy of males and the in­fants pre­sent in the group. How­ever, there are re­ports of higher death rates of in­fants fol­low­ing a change in the male mem­ber­ship of the group. It is pro­posed that this is a re­sult of in­fan­ti­cide by the in­com­ing male to clear out un­re­lated males and bring fe­males into es­trus sooner (Marsh, 1979). Though in­fan­ti­cide has been ob­served in the Tana River red colobus mon­keys and other pri­mate species, the causal re­la­tion­ship of in­fan­ti­cide and its re­la­tion­ship to re­pro­duc­tion and ge­neal­ogy have yet to be fully de­ter­mined. (Marsh, 1979; New­ton and Dun­bar, 1994; Oates, 1994; Struh­saker, 1975)

  • Parental Investment
  • altricial
  • pre-fertilization
    • provisioning
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-hatching/birth
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • female
  • extended period of juvenile learning
  • inherits maternal/paternal territory

Lifes­pan/Longevity

Lifes­pan is dif­fi­cult to es­ti­mate be­cause very few stud­ies of these pri­mates have ever lasted more than one year, let alone for an en­tire life­time of one of these pri­mates. Fre­quently only in­fant mor­tal­ity is as­sessed. In one long term study the av­er­age in­fant/small ju­ve­nile mor­tal­ity was, on av­er­age 0.064 deaths per in­fant per month. In­fant mor­tal­ity within the first year was as high as 55% for the pop­u­la­tion sam­pled. This same study found a higher mor­tal­ity for males in the group than fe­males, pos­si­bly from vi­o­lent con­flicts be­tween group males and ex­tra-group males for mat­ing ac­cess and re­sources. (Marsh, 1979)

Other colobine mon­keys rarely live longer than 30 years in cap­tiv­ity. It is likely that P. ru­fomi­tra­tus is like other mem­bers of the fam­ily Colobi­nae in this re­spect. Lifes­pans are likely to be even shorter in the wild. (Nowak, 1999)

Be­hav­ior

Like most pri­mates, P. ru­fomi­tra­tus is a noisy, so­cial an­i­mal that lives in het­ero­sex­ual groups. Typ­i­cally, red colobus mon­keys live in groups with be­tween 30 to 50 mem­bers, in­clud­ing mul­ti­ple males and fe­males (Oates, 1994). How­ever, in the re­duced and frag­mented habi­tat of the Tana River, the num­ber of in­di­vid­u­als in each group is much smaller, rang­ing from 12 to 30 (Marsh, 1979). In stark con­trast to the rain for­est species, Tana River red colobus often have only one or two males in the group. These de­mo­graph­ics may give males greater ac­cess to mates and also in­di­rectly re­duc­ing the stress on the lim­ited re­sources of their frag­mented habi­tat. The causes and im­pli­ca­tions of the small and frag­mented Tana River habi­tat on be­hav­ior are still being in­ves­ti­gated, but the dif­fer­ences be­tween P. ru­fomi­tra­tus at Tana (small group size, low # of males) and the rain for­est species are quite stark and fas­ci­nat­ing.

The daily time bud­get of red colobus mon­keys at Tana River dif­fers sig­nif­i­cantly from the rain-for­est species. They spend on av­er­age, the ma­jor­ity of their day rest­ing (47.8%) or feed­ing (30.0%), and the rest of the time mov­ing (7.2%), groom­ing (4.1%), or at play (3.4%). Feed­ing usu­ally oc­curs dur­ing the early morn­ing or late af­ter­noon. This is typ­i­cally fol­lowed by in­ac­tiv­ity dur­ing the mid­dle of the day and late evening. Though P.​rufomi­tra­tus is mostly ac­tive dur­ing the day, ob­ser­va­tions of other red colobus species have in­di­cated that some call­ing and sex­ual ac­tiv­ity occur at night.

Like many pri­mates, P. ru­fomi­tra­tus often en­gages in so­cial be­hav­iors such as play, ag­gres­sion, and groom­ing (Struh­saker, 1975). Play can en­tail any­thing from chas­ing and wrestling to bounc­ing off of branches. Ag­o­nis­tic be­hav­iors may be minor,such as a gri­mace, touch, or sup­plan­ta­tion. These are often as­so­ci­ated with a va­ri­ety of body and limb pos­tur­ing. Major ag­o­nis­tic be­hav­iors often in­clude stares and gapes usu­ally with lung­ing, slap­ping or branch shak­ing, and in ex­treme cases, phys­i­cal vi­o­lence. These ag­o­nis­tic be­hav­iors often occur be­tween males in re­la­tion­ship to dom­i­nance and group sta­tus. In gen­eral, males groom each other more often than fe­males, though the full ef­fects of this be­hav­ior have not been ex­am­ined for the small num­ber of males in the Tana River red colobus species (Struh­saker and Le­land, 1987). This pat­tern of male groom­ing is thought to arise be­cause of the pa­tri­lin­eal or­ga­ni­za­tion. (Marsh, 1979; Oates, 1994; Struh­saker and Le­land, 1987; Struh­saker, 1975)

Home Range

Pil­io­colobus ru­fomi­tra­tus ex­hibits smaller home ranges than other red colobus mon­keys (Marsh, 1979). The av­er­age an­nual home range of P. ru­fomi­tra­tus is near 9 ha com­pared to that of 34 to 114 ha for their coun­ter­parts. The av­er­age day range is 603 me­ters.

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

Like many pri­mates, P. ru­fomi­tra­tus often en­gages in so­cial be­hav­iors such as play, ag­gres­sion, and groom­ing (Struh­saker, 1975). Play can en­tail any­thing from chas­ing and wrestling to bounc­ing off of branches. Ag­o­nis­tic be­hav­iors may be minor, such as a gri­mace, touch, or sup­plan­ta­tion. These are often as­so­ci­ated with a va­ri­ety of body and limb pos­tur­ing. Major ag­o­nis­tic be­hav­iors often in­clude stares and gapes usu­ally with lung­ing, slap­ping or branch shak­ing, and in ex­treme cases, phys­i­cal vi­o­lence. These ag­o­nis­tic be­hav­iors often occur be­tween males in re­la­tion­ship to dom­i­nance and group sta­tus. In gen­eral, males groom each other more often than fe­males, though the full ef­fects of this be­hav­ior have not been ex­am­ined for the small num­ber of males in the Tana River red colobus species (Struh­saker and Le­land, 1987). This pat­tern of male groom­ing is thought to arise be­cause of the pa­tri­lin­eal or­ga­ni­za­tion. (Nowak, 1999; Struh­saker and Le­land, 1987)

Food Habits

Pil­io­colobus ru­fomi­tra­tus is pri­mar­ily a fo­li­vore, and this is re­lated to a com­ple­ment of mor­pho­log­i­cal adap­ta­tions to this kind of diet. All colobine mon­keys are dis­tin­guished from other old world pri­mates in that they posses a ru­mi­nant-like stom­ach rather than a sim­ple stom­ach. This stom­ach con­sists of a large sac­cu­lated cham­ber with a forestom­ach al­ka­line/acid sys­tem de­signed to di­gest plant mat­ter using bac­te­r­ial fer­men­ta­tion (Chivers, 1994). These pri­mates also have high cusped, ridged mo­lars de­signed to shear and fold leaves and seeds. This helps break down cell walls and ex­tract the nu­tri­ents con­tained within (Teaford and Lucas, 1994).

Pil­io­colobus ru­fomi­tra­tus is pri­mar­ily a fo­li­vore, though it feeds on a va­ri­ety of other plant mat­ter in­clud­ing fruits and flow­ers. In a study of diet choice, Tana River red colobus fed not only on young leaves (36%), but fruit and seeds (25%, mostly large and un­ripe), leaf buds (16.4%), ma­ture leaves (11.5%) and flow­ers (6.2%). The top three plant species eaten were Ficus syco­morus (29.4%), Sorindeia ob­tusi­fo­li­o­lata (19.6%) and Aca­cia ro­busta (15.0%). These species con­tribute the ma­jor­ity of the diet of Tana River red colobus mon­keys (Marsh, 1981a; Marsh, 1981c).

Stud­ies of the im­pact of plant phy­to­chem­istry on di­etary choice have found that the young leaves pre­dom­i­nately cho­sen are higher in ni­tro­gen (pro­tein) con­tent and lower in hard-to-di­gest fiber than the more abun­dant, ma­ture leaves. Thus P. ru­fomi­tra­tus re­lies mostly on sea­sonal young leaves, flow­ers and fruits when avail­able and re­verts to ma­ture leaves when nec­es­sary. Pil­io­colobus ru­fomi­tra­tus rarely eats the leaves of Diopy­ros mespili­formis de­spite that fact that this is the most com­mon tree in their habi­tat. This might be due to the sec­ondary com­pound naptho­quinone, a phe­no­lic toxin pro­duced in the leaves (Mowry et al., 1996). (Chivers, 1994; Marsh, 1981a; Marsh, 1981c; Mowry, et al., 1996; Teaford and Lucas, 1994)

  • Plant Foods
  • leaves
  • seeds, grains, and nuts
  • fruit
  • flowers

Pre­da­tion

Pil­io­colobus ru­fomi­tra­tus has very few preda­tors due to its large size and ar­bo­real lifestyle. Pre­sum­ably the pelage, dark red dor­sally and lighter ven­trally, helps to cam­ou­flage the shape of these mon­keys when viewed from above and below, blend­ing them into the for­est branches when viewed from above and the sky and canopy when viewed from below.

In the rain for­est species of red colobus, where pre­da­tion pres­sures from chim­panzee hunt­ing are sig­nif­i­cant, these an­i­mals often ex­hibit male-male bond­ing, swamp­ing and ag­gres­sion against chim­panzee hunt­ing par­ties (Struh­saker, 1975). How­ever, Tana River red colobus, which do not ex­pe­ri­ence this pres­sure, do not ex­hibit these be­hav­iors. Alarm call­ing in the form of a loud bark is a com­mon de­fense against pre­da­tion. Un­for­tu­nately they have few adap­ta­tions against their pri­mary preda­tors, hu­mans. Frag­men­ta­tion and re­duc­tion of habi­tat ex­poses Tana River red colobus to bush­meat traders (Oates, 1994). (Oates, 1994; Struh­saker and Le­land, 1987)

  • Known Predators
    • humans (Homo sapiens)
    • crowned hawk-eagles (Stephaboaetus coronatus)
    • chimpanzees (Pan)

Ecosys­tem Roles

Pil­io­colobus ru­fomi­tra­tus is host to a va­ri­ety of par­a­sites, though the exact num­ber and species are not known. They also may in­flu­ence plant growth and flow­er­ing pat­terns in their food species as some plant species may bloom syn­chro­nously to limit the im­pact of P. ru­fomi­tra­tus feed­ing. (Oates, 1994)

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Pos­i­tive

Tana River red colobus could be used for eco­tourism. Al­though they are not as sought after by tourists as are the great apes, they are nonethe­less a very promi­nent and in­ter­est­ing species. They are also often used as a source of food for hu­mans in the bush meat trade. (Med­ley, 1993; Struh­saker, 1975)

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Neg­a­tive

There are no known ad­verse ef­fects of P. ru­fomi­tra­tus on hu­mans.

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

Tana River red colobus mon­keys are listed as crit­i­cally en­dan­gered by IUCN be­cause their ex­tremely frag­mented habi­tat is under the con­stant threat of human en­croach­ment (IUCN Web­site, 2001). The small tract of for­est sur­round­ing the river is pri­mar­ily ex­ploited for agri­cul­ture uses but also for canoe con­struc­tion. Cat­tle feed­ing, in­ten­tional burns to in­crease grass pro­duc­tion, and dam con­struc­tion have also detri­men­tally al­tered the habi­tat on the Tana River.

These pri­mates pre­fer in­te­rior for­est habi­tat (Med­ley, 1993). Al­ter­ations to the for­est that re­sult in frag­men­ta­tion, re­duc­tion of for­est area and in­crease pe­riph­eral ex­po­sure ad­versely af­fect pop­u­la­tions. A pop­u­la­tion cen­sus found that be­tween 1975 and 1985, pop­u­la­tions de­clined by 80% mostly due to these habi­tat loss (Decker & Kin­naird, 1992). Con­ser­va­tion ef­forts should focus on main­tain­ing the in­tegrity of the in­te­rior for­est and re­duc­ing the causal fac­tors of frag­men­ta­tion. (Decker and Kin­naird, 1992; IUCN, 2000; Med­ley, 1993)

Other Com­ments

Pil­io­colobus ru­fomi­tra­tus was pre­vi­ously con­sid­ered a sub­species of a more wide­spread species of red colobus mon­key (Nowak, 1999; Oates, 1994). They were also pre­vi­ously rec­og­nized under the name Pro­colobus ru­fomi­tra­tus. (Nowak, 1999; Oates, 1994)

Con­trib­u­tors

Nancy Shef­ferly (ed­i­tor), An­i­mal Di­ver­sity Web.

Je­remy Jones (au­thor), Uni­ver­sity of Michi­gan-Ann Arbor, On­drej Pod­laha (ed­i­tor), Uni­ver­sity of Michi­gan-Ann Arbor.

Glossary

Ethiopian

living in sub-Saharan Africa (south of 30 degrees north) and Madagascar.

World Map

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

altricial

young are born in a relatively underdeveloped state; they are unable to feed or care for themselves or locomote independently for a period of time after birth/hatching. In birds, naked and helpless after hatching.

arboreal

Referring to an animal that lives in trees; tree-climbing.

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

diurnal
  1. active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
dominance hierarchies

ranking system or pecking order among members of a long-term social group, where dominance status affects access to resources or mates

ecotourism

humans benefit economically by promoting tourism that focuses on the appreciation of natural areas or animals. Ecotourism implies that there are existing programs that profit from the appreciation of natural areas or animals.

endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

fertilization

union of egg and spermatozoan

folivore

an animal that mainly eats leaves.

food

A substance that provides both nutrients and energy to a living thing.

forest

forest biomes are dominated by trees, otherwise forest biomes can vary widely in amount of precipitation and seasonality.

frugivore

an animal that mainly eats fruit

herbivore

An animal that eats mainly plants or parts of plants.

iteroparous

offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

polygynous

having more than one female as a mate at one time

riparian

Referring to something living or located adjacent to a waterbody (usually, but not always, a river or stream).

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

social

associates with others of its species; forms social groups.

tactile

uses touch to communicate

terrestrial

Living on the ground.

tropical

the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.

visual

uses sight to communicate

viviparous

reproduction in which fertilization and development take place within the female body and the developing embryo derives nourishment from the female.

year-round breeding

breeding takes place throughout the year

Ref­er­ences

Chivers, D. 1994. Func­tional Mor­phol­ogy of the Gas­troinesti­nal Tract. Pp. 205-228 in J Oates, A Davies, eds. Colobine Mon­keys: Their Ecol­ogy, Be­hav­iour and Evo­lu­tion. Cam­bridge: Cam­bridge Uni­ver­sity Press.

Decker, B., M. Kin­naird. 1992. Tana River Red Colobus and Crested Mangabey: Re­sults of re­cent cen­suses. Amer­i­can Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 26: 47-52.

IUCN, 2000. "IUCN Web­site" (On-line). Ac­cessed No­vem­ber 18, 2001 at http://​www.​redlist.​org/​search/​details.​php?​species=39993.

Karere, G., N. Oguge, J. Ki­rathe, P. Muo­ria, N. Moinde, M. Sule­man. 2004. Pop­u­la­tion Sizes and Dis­tri­b­u­tion of Pri­mates in the Lower Tana River Forests, Kenya. In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 25: 351-365. Ac­cessed Jan­u­ary 07, 2004 at http://​www.​ingentaconnect.​com/​content/​klu/​ijop/​2004/​00000025/​00000002/​00479983.

Marsh, C. 1979. Com­par­a­tive as­pects of so­cial or­ga­ni­za­tion in the Tana River red colobus. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsy­cholo­gie, 51: 337-362.

Marsh, C. 1981a. Diet choice among Red colobus (*Colobus ba­dius ru­fomi­tra­tus*) on the Tana River, Kenya. Folia Pri­ma­to­log­ica, 35: 147-178.

Marsh, C. 1981c. Rang­ing be­hav­iour and its re­la­tion to diet se­lec­tion in Tana River Red colobus. Jour­nal of Zo­ol­ogy (Lon­don), 195: 473-492.

Marsh, C. 1981b. Time bud­get of Tana River Red Colobus. Folia Pri­ma­to­log­ica, 35: 30-50.

Med­ley, K. 1993. Pri­mate con­ser­va­tion along the Tana River, Kenya: An ex­am­i­na­tion of the for­est habi­tat. Con­ser­va­tion Bi­ol­ogy, 7/1: 109-121.

Mowry, C., B. Decker, D. Shure. 1996. The role of phy­to­chem­istry in di­etary choices of the Tana River Red Colobus Mon­keys. In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, 17(1): 63-81.

New­ton, P., R. Dun­bar. 1994. Colobine Mon­key So­ci­ety. Pp. 311-346 in J Oates, A Davies, eds. Colobine Mon­keys: Their Ecol­ogy, Be­hav­iour and Evo­lu­tion. Cam­bridge: Cam­bridge Uni­ver­sity Press.

Nowak, R. 1999. Walker's Pri­mates of the World. Bal­ti­more: Johns Hop­kins Uni­ver­sity Press.

Oates, J. 1994. The nat­ural his­tory of African colobines. Pp. 75-128 in J Oates, A Davies, eds. Colobine Mon­keys: Their Ecol­ogy Be­hav­iour and Evo­lu­tion. Cam­bridge: Cam­bridge Uni­ver­sity Press.

Struh­saker, T. 1975. The Red Colobus Mon­key. Chicago: Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

Struh­saker, T., L. Le­land. 1987. Colobines: In­fan­ti­cide by Adult Males. Pp. 83-97 in B Smuts, D Ch­eney, R Sey­farth, R Wrang­ham, T Struh­saker, eds. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. Chicago: The Uni­ver­sity of Chicago Press.

Teaford, M., P. Lucas. 1994. Func­tional Mor­phol­ogy of colobine teeth. Pp. 173-204 in J Oates, A Davies, eds. Colobine Mon­keys: Their Ecol­ogy, Be­hav­iour and Evo­lu­tion. Cam­bridge: Cam­bridge Uni­ver­sity Press.