Gorilla beringeieastern gorilla

Ge­o­graphic Range

Moun­tain, or east­ern go­ril­las, Go­rilla beringei, are found in the Virunga vol­ca­noes that sep­a­rate the De­mo­c­ra­tic Re­pub­lic of Congo from Rwanda and Uganda.

Habi­tat

Moun­tain go­ril­las in­habit the mon­tane cloud for­est of the Virunga range. Oc­ca­sion­ally they go into the afro-alpine mead­ows (4,000 m) where tem­per­a­tures are sub­freez­ing at night and there is lit­tle suit­able food to for­age on.

  • Range elevation
    4000 (high) m
    13123.36 (high) ft

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

Go­ril­las are the largest pri­mate, with av­er­age lengths of 150 cm for fe­males and 185 cm for males. They are highly sex­u­ally di­mor­phic, with fe­males weigh­ing 70 to 114 kg and males av­er­ag­ing 160 kg. They have ro­bust bod­ies, long mus­cu­lar arms, short legs, mas­sive heads, and males have large, sharp ca­nine teeth. Moun­tain go­rilla coats are silky and long, rang­ing in color from blue-black to brown­ish-grey. Ma­ture males de­velop a large patch of sil­ver or grey hair on their backs, giv­ing them the name sil­ver­backs. Males also have apoc­rine glands in their armpits that emit a strong odor when the an­i­mal is under stress.

Moun­tain go­ril­las dif­fer from other go­ril­las in hav­ing longer hair, larger jaws and teeth, smaller nose, and shorter arms.

  • Sexual Dimorphism
  • male larger
  • Range mass
    70 to 200 kg
    154.19 to 440.53 lb
  • Range length
    150 to 185 cm
    59.06 to 72.83 in

Re­pro­duc­tion

Moun­tain go­ril­las are polyg­y­nous; the dom­i­nant male in each group has ex­clu­sive ac­cess to all the fe­males in the group.

Re­pro­duc­tive rates are slow and a fe­male may leave only 2 to 6 off­spring over a 40 year life-span. Males that have a harems of 3 to 4 fe­males in­crease their re­pro­duc­tive out­put by fa­ther­ing 10 to 20 off­spring over 50 years. These an­i­mals don't ma­ture sex­u­ally until well into their teens.

Mat­ing be­hav­ior is ini­ti­ated by the fe­male, with a se­ries of slow and hes­i­tant ap­proaches to the male. A fe­male is re­cep­tive only dur­ing es­trus, and she will cease to ovu­late for sev­eral years after giv­ing birth. The length of the es­trous cycle of a fe­male moun­tain go­rilla is 28 days, and there is no vis­i­ble ex­ter­nal men­strual flow.

A sin­gle, de­pen­dent young is born after a eight and a half month ges­ta­tion pe­riod. Wean­ing often doesn't occur until three years of age, and ju­ve­niles may re­main with moth­ers for years after that. Fe­males are sex­u­ally ma­ture by 10 years of age, but males are un­likely to start breed­ing be­fore 15 years. Re­pro­duc­tive out­put for fe­males is about one sur­viv­ing off­spring every 8 years (sur­vival im­ply­ing reach­ing breed­ing age).

  • Breeding interval
    The interval between reproductive events depends upon infant survival. Females are capable of producing an infant every 4 to 5 years.
  • Breeding season
    These animals breed throughout the year.
  • Average number of offspring
    1
  • Average gestation period
    8.5 months
  • Range weaning age
    36 to 48 months
  • Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    10 years
  • Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    15 years

Fe­males pro­vide most of the parental care in this species. Fe­males nurse and carry their young for about 4 years. They also play with the young, teach them, and groom them.

The role of males in parental care is less di­rect, al­though no less im­por­tant. Males pro­tect the fe­males and the young within their so­cial group from po­ten­tially in­fan­ti­ci­dal rival males who may take con­trol of the group.

  • Parental Investment
  • altricial
  • pre-fertilization
    • provisioning
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-hatching/birth
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • extended period of juvenile learning

Lifes­pan/Longevity

Go­ril­las can reach ages of 40 to 50 years.

  • Typical lifespan
    Status: wild
    50 (high) years

Be­hav­ior

The main so­cial unit is a male with a harem of fe­males and their off­spring. These groups are non­ter­ri­to­r­ial, but se­vere in­ter­group con­flicts can occur when groups en­counter each other and es­pe­cially if a lone male con­tacts the group. The dom­i­nant male of a group is mas­sive com­pared to the other mem­bers and they all defer to him.

Fe­males trans­fer from their natal group to a new group be­fore breed­ing. This gen­er­ally oc­curs at around 8 years of age. Often they join a lone male and start a new group, rather than join an es­tab­lished group and be a lower rank­ing fe­male. Males often leave the natal group at around 11 years of age. Males, how­ever, can't join an es­tab­lish group, and they spend much time in soli­tary ex­is­tence until they can gain fe­males and begin a group of their own at age 15 or older.

Groom­ing often oc­curs be­tween fe­males and males, or among fe­males.

Go­ril­las spend about 30% of the day feed­ing, 30% trav­el­ing, and 40% rest­ing. They make nests to sleep and rest in that can be in trees, on steep slopes or even on the ground.

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

All pri­mates have com­plex pat­terns of com­mu­ni­ca­tion. Go­ril­las are known to use vo­cal­iza­tions to com­mu­ni­cate with one an­other. Tac­tile com­mu­ni­ca­tion, in the form of groom­ing, play, and sex­ual con­tact, also oc­curs. Males emit a strong odor when stressed, which ap­pears to func­tion as a type of chem­i­cal com­mu­ni­ca­tion. In ad­di­tion to these, go­ril­las use body pos­tures and fa­cial ex­pres­sions, as well as other vi­sual sig­nals, to com­mu­ni­cate with one an­other.

Food Habits

Moun­tain go­ril­las oc­ca­sion­ally eat in­ver­te­brates, but they are pri­mar­ily fo­liv­o­rous. They eat the roots, leaves, stems, and pith of herbs, vines, shrubs, and bam­boo. Their diet is sup­ple­mented by small amounts of bark, wood, roots, flow­ers, fruit, fungi, ep­ithe­lium stripped from roots, galls, in­ver­te­brates, and go­rilla dung.

  • Animal Foods
  • insects
  • Plant Foods
  • leaves
  • roots and tubers
  • wood, bark, or stems
  • fruit
  • Other Foods
  • fungus
  • dung

Pre­da­tion

These an­i­mals are very large, and live in re­gions where not many po­ten­tial preda­tors exist. It is not likely that they fall prey to any par­tic­u­lar species with any reg­u­lar­ity.

Ecosys­tem Roles

These an­i­mals may be im­por­tant in struc­tur­ing plant com­mu­ni­ties, as they feed heav­ily on veg­e­ta­tion.

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Pos­i­tive

Go­ril­las may be vis­ited by eco­tourists, en­hanc­ing local economies.

  • Positive Impacts
  • body parts are source of valuable material
  • ecotourism
  • research and education

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Neg­a­tive

There is con­tin­u­ing po­lit­i­cal pres­sure to con­vert the re­main­ing go­rilla re­serves into areas for farm­ing or com­mer­cial use. Due to the high pop­u­la­tion den­sity, many peo­ple feel that the land would be bet­ter put to com­mer­cial use.

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

Moun­tain go­ril­las are highly en­dan­gered. This is due both to habi­tat de­struc­tion and se­vere poach­ing pres­sures. Go­rilla species are sub­jected to heavy pres­sure from poach­ing for body parts and for young an­i­mals col­lected for zoos and pri­vate col­lec­tions, gen­er­at­ing il­le­gal in­come. The civil war oc­cur­ring in the re­gion they in­habit has only added to their plight, in­creas­ing mor­tal­ity through ac­ci­dents and the break­down of pa­trol units against poach­ers.

Other Com­ments

Go­ril­las are usu­ally very gen­tle crea­tures. Their only ef­fec­tive preda­tors are hu­mans. They are often thought to be "slow" or "dumb" be­cause of their slug­gish­ness, but in fact they are in­tel­li­gent and ca­pa­ble of learn­ing sign lan­guage.

There is an on­go­ing de­bate as to the re­la­tion­ship of go­ril­las, chimps and hu­mans. Go­ril­las are clearly one of our clos­est rel­a­tives if not the clos­est, shar­ing 98% of our genome.

Con­trib­u­tors

Nancy Shef­ferly (ed­i­tor), An­i­mal Di­ver­sity Web.

Tracy Lind­s­ley (au­thor), Uni­ver­sity of Michi­gan-Ann Arbor, Anna Bess Sorin (au­thor), Bi­ol­ogy Dept., Uni­ver­sity of Mem­phis.

Glossary

Ethiopian

living in sub-Saharan Africa (south of 30 degrees north) and Madagascar.

World Map

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

altricial

young are born in a relatively underdeveloped state; they are unable to feed or care for themselves or locomote independently for a period of time after birth/hatching. In birds, naked and helpless after hatching.

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

diurnal
  1. active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
dominance hierarchies

ranking system or pecking order among members of a long-term social group, where dominance status affects access to resources or mates

ecotourism

humans benefit economically by promoting tourism that focuses on the appreciation of natural areas or animals. Ecotourism implies that there are existing programs that profit from the appreciation of natural areas or animals.

endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

fertilization

union of egg and spermatozoan

folivore

an animal that mainly eats leaves.

herbivore

An animal that eats mainly plants or parts of plants.

iteroparous

offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

mountains

This terrestrial biome includes summits of high mountains, either without vegetation or covered by low, tundra-like vegetation.

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

polygynous

having more than one female as a mate at one time

rainforest

rainforests, both temperate and tropical, are dominated by trees often forming a closed canopy with little light reaching the ground. Epiphytes and climbing plants are also abundant. Precipitation is typically not limiting, but may be somewhat seasonal.

sedentary

remains in the same area

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

social

associates with others of its species; forms social groups.

tactile

uses touch to communicate

terrestrial

Living on the ground.

tropical

the region of the earth that surrounds the equator, from 23.5 degrees north to 23.5 degrees south.

visual

uses sight to communicate

viviparous

reproduction in which fertilization and development take place within the female body and the developing embryo derives nourishment from the female.

year-round breeding

breeding takes place throughout the year

Ref­er­ences

Estes, R.D. 1991. The Be­hav­ior Guide to African Mam­mals. Uni­ver­sity of Cal­i­for­nia Press: Los An­ge­les.

Mit­ter­meier, Rus­sell A. and Dorothy L. Ch­eney. 1987. Con­ser­va­tion of Pri­mates and Their Habi­tats. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. 189- 202.

Watts, David P. 1994. Ag­nos­tic re­la­tion­ships be­tween fe­male moun­tain go­ril­las. Be­hav­ioral Eco­log­i­cal So­cio­bi­ol­ogy, vol.34. 347-358.

Watts, David P. 1990. Ecol­ogy of go­ril­las and its re­la­tion to fe­male trans­fer in moun­tain go­ril­las. In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Pri­ma­tol­ogy, vol.11. 21-43.

Stew­art, Kelly J. and Alexan­der H. Har­court. 1986. Go­ril­las: Vari­a­tion in Fe­male Re­la­tion­ships. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. 155-164.

Blaf­fer Hrdy, Sarah and Pa­tri­cia L. Whit­ten. 1986. Pat­tern­ing of Sex­ual Ac­tiv­ity. Pri­mate So­ci­eties. 370- 384.