Aptenodytes patagonicusking penguin

Ge­o­graphic Range

Apten­odytes patag­o­ni­cus (king pen­guins) colonies are mainly lo­cated on is­lands sur­round­ing Antarc­tica. Is­lands in­clude Crozet, Falk­land, Heard, Ker­gue­len, Mac­quarie, Prince Ed­ward, South Geor­gia and South Sand­wich. Al­though no colonies have been found south of lat­i­tude 60 de­grees S, some non-breed­ing mem­bers have taken res­i­dence in south­ern Chile and south­ern Ar­gentina. Some lone wan­der­ers have been found as far north as Brazil and South Africa and as far south as the Antarc­tic Coast.

On South Geor­gia Is­land over 30 colonies of A. patag­o­ni­cus patag­o­ni­cus re­side. Colony sizes range from ap­prox­i­mately 39,000 breed­ing pairs at both Sal­is­bury Plains and St. An­drews Bay to 9,000 pairs lo­cated at Royal Bay. This sub­species is only found on South Geor­gia and the Falk­land Is­lands. An­other ge­net­i­cally unique sub­species, A. patag­o­ni­cus halli, is only found on Crozet, Heard, Ker­gue­len, Mac­quarie and Prince Ed­wards is­lands. (Mc­Go­ni­gal and Wood­worth, 2001; Shir­i­hai, 2008)

Habi­tat

King pen­guins spend a lot of time in the ocean feed­ing, but their pri­mary habi­tats are sparsely veg­e­tated areas of is­lands in the south­ern oceans and sub-Antarc­tic. The is­lands south of the Polar Front are typ­i­cally more glaciated and at higher al­ti­tudes. For ex­am­ple, Heard Is­land is 2745 me­ters above sea level. These is­lands are still out of reach of the Antarc­tic pack ice, but icy con­di­tions are still preva­lent. As in the case of South Geor­gia, the bays freeze over and the is­land is over half cov­ered in ice dur­ing the win­ter months. In the lo­ca­tions not cov­ered in ice, bryophytes are the pri­mary veg­e­ta­tion. Is­lands like Mac­quarie (433 me­ters above sea level) have some flow­er­ing plants and ferns and the air tem­per­a­ture only varies a few de­grees be­tween the sum­mer and win­ter sea­sons.

While king pen­guins pre­fer to live on is­lands south of the Polar Front, they pre­fer to fish in wa­ters just north of it where sur­face air tem­per­a­tures are around 4.5 de­grees C. They have been known to dive to a max­i­mum of 322 me­ters. (Mc­Go­ni­gal and Wood­worth, 2001)

  • Range elevation
    433 to 2745 m
    1420.60 to 9005.91 ft
  • Range depth
    0 to 322 m
    0.00 to 1056.43 ft

Phys­i­cal De­scrip­tion

King pen­guins are the sec­ond largest of all pen­guin species. Fe­males are noted to be slightly smaller than males. How­ever, no spe­cific fe­male mea­sure­ments have been recorded. Their doc­u­mented height ranges from 85 to 95 cm and weight is be­tween 9.3 and 17.3 kg. Av­er­age adult weight has been found to be 11.8 kg.

Al­though they are eas­ily con­fused with em­peror pen­guins (Apten­odytes forsteri), king pen­guins are more col­or­ful and have a longer, more slen­der bill. This bill has a stripe on the lower mandible that ranges in color from pink­ish-red to or­ange-yel­low and ex­hibits ul­tra­vi­o­let (UV) re­flectance. The func­tion of this beak spot is un­clear, but it is thought to sig­nal sex­ual ma­tu­rity, health and/or so­cial stand­ing. The spot, with­out UV re­flectance, is found in ju­ve­niles. Nei­ther the beak spot nor UV re­flectance are seen in chicks. This UV re­flec­tive beak spot does not dif­fer be­tween sexes.

Adult king pen­guins have a dark, nearly black head with or­ange to or­ange-yel­low, spoon-shaped spots on ei­ther side of the head and an or­ange area that is most in­tense at the throat and fades down the upper breast into pale yel­low then fi­nally a white ven­tral side. The dor­sal side of the body and flip­pers con­sist of gray and black feath­ers with a sil­very sheen. The sides are sep­a­rated from the ven­tral side by a nar­row, black line. The front edge of the flip­per also has a black line that ex­tends to a black tip. King pen­guins are sex­u­ally monomor­phic in plumage, but males are slightly larger.

Ju­ve­nile king pen­guins are sim­i­lar to adults, but their col­oration isn't as vivid. They do not reach full adult col­oration until three years of age. Prior to the first molt, the chicks are sim­ply dark brown.

There is no ev­i­dence of phys­i­cal dif­fer­ences be­tween the two sub­species. (Jou­ventin, et al., 2005; Mc­Go­ni­gal and Wood­worth, 2001; Nolan, et al., 2010; Putz and Bost, 1994; Shir­i­hai, 2008)

  • Sexual Dimorphism
  • sexes alike
  • male larger
  • Range mass
    9.3 to 17.3 kg
    20.48 to 38.11 lb
  • Average mass
    11.8 kg
    25.99 lb
  • Range length
    85 to 95 cm
    33.46 to 37.40 in
  • Average basal metabolic rate
    25.889 W
    AnAge

Re­pro­duc­tion

King pen­guins have a lower rate of monogamy than smaller pen­guin species. Cur­rently, there is no de­fin­i­tive an­swer as to why this oc­curs; how­ever, two ex­pla­na­tions have merit: 1) mates not ar­riv­ing to the colony at the same time, and 2) the amount of fat the pen­guin has stored. Fat stor­age plays a role in the low monogamy rate be­cause if the pen­guins begin stor­ing fat too early they be­come more vul­ner­a­ble to preda­tors. If they begin stor­ing fat too late, they may not re­turn to the colony at the same time as their mate. If both mates do not ar­rive at the colony at the same time, breed­ing can be de­layed or a new mate may be cho­sen.

Fe­males ap­pear to be more se­lec­tive than males when choos­ing a mate, but both sexes seem to choose a mate based on their plumage. Early breed­ing pairs have higher ul­tra­vi­o­let re­flectance of beak spots than those breed­ing later in the sea­son. The plumage color on the breasts and au­ric­u­lar areas are thought to di­rectly re­flect the health of a king pen­guin’s im­mune sys­tem. The health­ier it is, the brighter the plumage.

Males ad­ver­tise for mates with a com­bi­na­tion of vo­cal­iza­tions and vi­sual dis­plays. Male king pen­guins pro­duce a trum­pet­ing call and then stretch to full height with bills raised. Once a fe­male ac­cepts, the two face each other and con­tinue to en­gage in phys­i­cal dis­plays in­clud­ing strut­ting, bow­ing, shak­ing, call­ing, and stretch­ing to max­i­mum height with bills in the air. (Bried, et al., 1999; Nolan, et al., 2010)

King pen­guins breed yearly on the flat shore­lines of the sub-Antarc­tic is­lands. Their cycle be­ings with a 1-month molt­ing stage for both par­ents, which is com­plete by the end of Oc­to­ber. Once the molt is com­plete, the courtship stage can last for just over a month. The fe­male lays a sin­gle, green­ish-white egg in No­vem­ber or De­cem­ber. This is trans­ferred to the male pen­guin's feet and is in­cu­bated for ap­prox­i­mately 54 days under a pouch of belly skin that keeps it at the pen­guin’s in­ter­nal body tem­per­a­ture. After lay­ing the egg, the fe­male leaves to feed and re­plen­ish the weight that was lost. When the fe­male re­turns, the part­ners take turns in in­cu­bat­ing the egg, with shifts rang­ing from 5 to 22 days. The av­er­age birth mass for king pen­guin chicks is 430 g. Post-hatch­ing, the par­ents con­tinue tak­ing turns to in­cu­bate and feed the chick until May, when it is big enough to sur­vive on its own.

By May, the chicks are fairly in­de­pen­dent. They stay with the colony and sur­vive off their stored fat until the fol­low­ing Oc­to­ber. In these months, both par­ents leave to for­age and re­turn pe­ri­od­i­cally to feed their chick. Dur­ing that time the chicks live in crèches (groups of ju­ve­niles) until they have gained enough weight to be­come com­pletely in­de­pen­dent. Birth to in­de­pen­dence takes 14 to 16 months. Ju­ve­nile king pen­guins do not reach re­pro­duc­tive ma­tu­rity until 3 to 5 years of age.

Suc­cess­ful par­ents do not begin their next breed­ing cycle until their chick has suc­cess­fully fledged. This causes a suc­cess­ful breed­ing pair to begin the next sea­son late. The out­come is usu­ally a failed cycle be­cause an egg laid after De­cem­ber typ­i­cally is not suc­cess­ful. How­ever, this fail­ure al­lows them to breed ear­lier the fol­low­ing sea­son. The ear­lier that the breed­ing cycle be­gins the more likely it is to be suc­cess­ful. This bi­en­nial pat­tern to their breed­ing cycle makes king pen­guin re­pro­duc­tion unique.

Not all breed­ing pairs in a colony are on the same bi­en­nial cycle, and not all are guar­an­teed to fol­low the suc­cess-fail-suc­cess-fail pat­tern. It is most likely for them to fol­low this pat­tern or a suc­cess-fail-fail-suc­cess pat­tern. Some are on al­ter­nat­ing cy­cles so that there are chicks born dur­ing every breed­ing sea­son.

It is be­lieved that food avail­abil­ity plays a role in the tim­ing and suc­cess rate of the breed­ing cycle be­cause it di­rectly af­fects the health of the par­ent. (Aubin and Jou­ventin, 1998; Bried, et al., 1999; Cote, 2000; Dob­son and Jou­ventin, 2003; Lock­ley, 1984; Nico­laus, et al., 2007; Ols­son, 1996; Shir­i­hai, 2008)

  • Breeding interval
    King penguins breed once yearly, but normally are successful twice over 3 calendar years.
  • Breeding season
    King penguins breed between October and December.
  • Average eggs per season
    1
  • Average time to hatching
    54 days
  • Range fledging age
    14 to 16 months
  • Range time to independence
    14 to 16 months
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    3 (low) years
  • Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (female)
    5 years
  • Range age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    3 (low) years
  • Average age at sexual or reproductive maturity (male)
    5 years

King pen­guin par­ents highly in­vest in their re­pro­duc­tive cy­cles. The males must begin the cycle with enough fat stored to sus­tain them through courtship, egg-lay­ing and the first part of in­cu­ba­tion. By the end of their first in­cu­ba­tion shift the males have typ­i­cally lost 30 per­cent of their body weight. A min­i­mum body mass of 10 kg is con­sid­ered to be a crit­i­cal mass for male king pen­guins. When they are ap­proach­ing 10 kg, the males must choose whether to aban­don the egg or to con­tinue wait­ing for the fe­male to re­turn and re­lieve them. Hatch­lings are semi-al­tri­cial, and there­fore have con­sid­er­able de­vel­op­ment to achieve post-hatch­ing. This re­quires a large parental in­vest­ment to brood and nour­ish the young. (Ols­son, 1996; Ols­son, 1997)

  • Parental Investment
  • altricial
  • male parental care
  • female parental care
  • pre-fertilization
    • provisioning
    • protecting
      • female
  • pre-hatching/birth
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-weaning/fledging
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female
  • pre-independence
    • provisioning
      • male
      • female
    • protecting
      • male
      • female

Lifes­pan/Longevity

Apten­odytes patag­o­ni­cus has been gener­i­cally doc­u­mented as a long-lived bird. How­ever, no nu­mer­i­cal lifes­pan data has been pub­lished. Cap­tive king pen­guins can live up to 26 years. (Flower, 1938)

  • Range lifespan
    Status: captivity
    26 (high) years
  • Average lifespan
    Status: captivity
    26 years
    AnAge

Be­hav­ior

King pen­guins are one of only two pen­guin species that do not build nests. In­stead of build­ing a nest, king pen­guins carry their eggs and young around on their feet dur­ing de­vel­op­ment. The lack of a nest also adds to the noise level of the colonies be­cause they use their unique voice sys­tem to call to both their young and their mates.

King pen­guins are very so­cial birds, and live in colonies with pop­u­la­tions of up to 39,000 breed­ing pairs. The colonies are gen­er­ally found on large, sparsely veg­e­tated areas of the is­lands they in­habit be­cause king pen­guins avoid climb­ing. They have been seen hud­dling to­gether dur­ing bliz­zards to con­serve heat. Large colonies have com­mu­nal crèches, or groups, of chicks that stay to­gether for pro­tec­tion while both par­ents are si­mul­ta­ne­ously for­ag­ing.

They have been known to travel up to 500 km from their breed­ing grounds to for­age, and will bathe in the ocean be­fore fish­ing.

The so­cial hi­er­ar­chies of king pen­guin colonies are not en­tirely un­der­stood. The pen­guins com­pete for po­si­tions in the cen­ter of the colonies, and these birds are more ag­gres­sive and more suc­cess­ful at re­pro­duc­tion than ones fur­ther out. It is thought to pos­si­bly re­late to the ul­tra­vi­o­let beak spots or plumage. How­ever, no de­fin­i­tive con­clu­sions have been made. (Aubin and Jou­ventin, 1998; Dob­son and Jou­ventin, 2003; Lock­ley, 1984; Mc­Go­ni­gal and Wood­worth, 2001; Shir­i­hai, 2008; Viera, et al., 2007; de Vil­liers and de Bruyn, 2004)

  • Average territory size
    0.5 m^2

Home Range

Breed­ing pairs de­fend an area that is ap­prox­i­mately 0.5 square me­ters against neigh­bor­ing king pen­guins and preda­tors.

Com­mu­ni­ca­tion and Per­cep­tion

The pri­mary form of com­mu­ni­ca­tion in king pen­guins is a two-voice sys­tem that is pro­duced by the sy­rinx, a two-part organ lo­cated where the bronchi join. Each part pro­duces sound in­de­pen­dently. In pen­guins, the sy­rinx is only found in the genus Apten­odytes. The UV re­flec­tive beak spots may be used for sig­nal­ing, but the true use is un­known.

Due to the noisy en­vi­ron­ment of king pen­guin colonies, adults re­peat­edly call out 3 to 7 syl­la­bles of vary­ing vol­umes with two fre­quen­cies to lo­cate chicks. The chicks are thought to iden­tify the calls by the lower of the two fre­quen­cies be­cause they trans­mit far­ther in the seem­ing chaos of many adults call­ing at once. The higher fre­quency has no doc­u­mented use. This abil­ity has been termed "cock­tail-party ef­fect." The parental call is thought to be learned dur­ing the first five weeks of life and is im­por­tant be­cause of the lack of nests and land­marks in the king pen­guins' habi­tats.

At this time com­mu­ni­ca­tion re­search is mainly fo­cused on the par­ent/off­spring con­nec­tion, but it is be­lieved that the two-voice sys­tems are also used to lo­cate mates.

King pen­guins also in­cor­po­rate move­ments and be­hav­iors into courtship rit­u­als. Males will pro­duce trum­pet­ing calls and stretch to their max­i­mum height to at­tract mates. Once a fe­male ac­cepts, the two will stand fac­ing each other and will en­gage in a se­ries of stretch­ing, bob­bing, shak­ing, bow­ing, and call­ing. Like all birds, king pen­guins per­ceive their en­vi­ron­ments through vi­sual, au­di­tory, tac­tile and chem­i­cal stim­uli. (Aubin and Jou­ventin, 1998; Aubin, et al., 2000; Jou­ventin, et al., 2005; Jou­ventin, et al., 1999)

Food Habits

Apten­odytes patag­o­ni­cus trav­els up to 500 km from its colony to the ocean to feed on cephalopods, small fish, and squid. They can re­main un­der­wa­ter ap­prox­i­mately 10 min­utes and reach speeds of 12 km per hour while div­ing 25 to 322 m. Adults take turns re­turn­ing at ir­reg­u­lar in­ter­vals to sup­ple­ment the young that stay in the breed­ing grounds. Dur­ing their first win­ter the chicks pre­dom­i­nantly sur­vive from their stored fat. (Aubin and Jou­ventin, 1998; Lock­ley, 1984; Mc­Go­ni­gal and Wood­worth, 2001; Shir­i­hai, 2008)

  • Animal Foods
  • fish
  • mollusks
  • other marine invertebrates

Pre­da­tion

Antarc­tic fur seals, sub-Antarc­tic fur seals, leop­ard seals, and killer whales reg­u­larly prey on adult king pen­guins. Also, brown skuas and giant pe­trels prey on king pen­guin chicks. (Char­bon­nier, et al., 2009; Em­slie, et al., 1995)

Ecosys­tem Roles

King pen­guins oc­ca­sion­ally serve as prey for Antarc­tic fur seals (Arc­to­cephalus gazelle), sub­antarc­tic fur seals (Arc­to­cephalus trop­i­calis), brown skuas (Cathar­acta lonnbergi) and giant pe­trels (Macronectes gi­gan­teus). None of these preda­tors ap­pear to con­trol the king pen­guin pop­u­la­tion be­cause they are not a pri­mary food source, and the king pen­guin pop­u­la­tion is steadily grow­ing. King pen­guins act as preda­tors for cephalopods, small fish and squid found in their ge­o­graphic range, but there is no doc­u­men­ta­tion stat­ing if the pop­u­la­tions are con­trolled by this pre­da­tion. (Aubin and Jou­ventin, 1998; Char­bon­nier, et al., 2009; Em­slie, et al., 1995; Lock­ley, 1984; Mc­Go­ni­gal and Wood­worth, 2001; Shir­i­hai, 2008)

The hard tick, Ixodes uriae, is a par­a­site that in­fests king pen­guins. The mor­tal­ity rate of adult king pen­guins due to hard ticks is un­known; how­ever, there is doc­u­men­ta­tion of death due to hy­per­in­fes­ta­tion of hard ticks. A bird louse, Aus­tro­go­niodes pau­liani, is also a com­mon par­a­site found on these pen­guins. (Gau­thier-Clerc, et al., 1998)

Com­men­sal/Par­a­sitic Species
  • Hard ticks (Ixodes uriae)
  • Bird louse (Aus­tro­go­niodes pau­liani)

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Pos­i­tive

King pen­guins are part of the eco­tourism busi­ness. Dur­ing a study of na­ture-based tourism on the Falk­land Is­lands, king pen­guins were the most pop­u­lar of the pen­guins in the area. All of the tourists vis­ited the king pen­guin colony, ver­sus only half of the tourists vis­it­ing the sec­ond most pop­u­lar pen­guins species. The tourists also stayed to view the king pen­guins for the longest time frame at a mean of 63 min­utes ver­sus only 19 min­utes for the sec­ond most pop­u­lar pen­guin species. (Otley, 2005)

Eco­nomic Im­por­tance for Hu­mans: Neg­a­tive

There are no known neg­a­tive eco­nomic ef­fects of king pen­guins on hu­mans.

Con­ser­va­tion Sta­tus

The IUCN Red List states that king pen­guins have a sta­tus of Least Con­cern based on the fol­low­ing cri­te­ria: ge­o­graphic range is greater than 20,000 square kilo­me­ters, pop­u­la­tion size is greater than 10,000 in­di­vid­u­als, and the pop­u­la­tion size seems to be in­creas­ing.

Con­trib­u­tors

Keena Ed­wards (au­thor), Rad­ford Uni­ver­sity, Karen Pow­ers (ed­i­tor), Rad­ford Uni­ver­sity, Rachelle Ster­ling (ed­i­tor), Spe­cial Pro­jects.

Glossary

Antarctica

lives on Antarctica, the southernmost continent which sits astride the southern pole.

acoustic

uses sound to communicate

altricial

young are born in a relatively underdeveloped state; they are unable to feed or care for themselves or locomote independently for a period of time after birth/hatching. In birds, naked and helpless after hatching.

bilateral symmetry

having body symmetry such that the animal can be divided in one plane into two mirror-image halves. Animals with bilateral symmetry have dorsal and ventral sides, as well as anterior and posterior ends. Synapomorphy of the Bilateria.

carnivore

an animal that mainly eats meat

chemical

uses smells or other chemicals to communicate

colonial

used loosely to describe any group of organisms living together or in close proximity to each other - for example nesting shorebirds that live in large colonies. More specifically refers to a group of organisms in which members act as specialized subunits (a continuous, modular society) - as in clonal organisms.

diurnal
  1. active during the day, 2. lasting for one day.
dominance hierarchies

ranking system or pecking order among members of a long-term social group, where dominance status affects access to resources or mates

ecotourism

humans benefit economically by promoting tourism that focuses on the appreciation of natural areas or animals. Ecotourism implies that there are existing programs that profit from the appreciation of natural areas or animals.

endothermic

animals that use metabolically generated heat to regulate body temperature independently of ambient temperature. Endothermy is a synapomorphy of the Mammalia, although it may have arisen in a (now extinct) synapsid ancestor; the fossil record does not distinguish these possibilities. Convergent in birds.

female parental care

parental care is carried out by females

iteroparous

offspring are produced in more than one group (litters, clutches, etc.) and across multiple seasons (or other periods hospitable to reproduction). Iteroparous animals must, by definition, survive over multiple seasons (or periodic condition changes).

male parental care

parental care is carried out by males

monogamous

Having one mate at a time.

motile

having the capacity to move from one place to another.

natatorial

specialized for swimming

native range

the area in which the animal is naturally found, the region in which it is endemic.

oceanic islands

islands that are not part of continental shelf areas, they are not, and have never been, connected to a continental land mass, most typically these are volcanic islands.

oviparous

reproduction in which eggs are released by the female; development of offspring occurs outside the mother's body.

pelagic

An aquatic biome consisting of the open ocean, far from land, does not include sea bottom (benthic zone).

piscivore

an animal that mainly eats fish

saltwater or marine

mainly lives in oceans, seas, or other bodies of salt water.

seasonal breeding

breeding is confined to a particular season

sedentary

remains in the same area

sexual

reproduction that includes combining the genetic contribution of two individuals, a male and a female

tactile

uses touch to communicate

temperate

that region of the Earth between 23.5 degrees North and 60 degrees North (between the Tropic of Cancer and the Arctic Circle) and between 23.5 degrees South and 60 degrees South (between the Tropic of Capricorn and the Antarctic Circle).

terrestrial

Living on the ground.

tundra

A terrestrial biome with low, shrubby or mat-like vegetation found at extremely high latitudes or elevations, near the limit of plant growth. Soils usually subject to permafrost. Plant diversity is typically low and the growing season is short.

visual

uses sight to communicate

Ref­er­ences

Aubin, T., P. Jou­ventin. 1998. Cock­tail-party ef­fect in king pen­guin colonies. Pro­ceed­ings of the Royal So­ci­ety B: Bi­o­log­i­cal Sci­ences, 265: 1665-1673.

Aubin, T., P. Jou­ventin, C. Hilde­brand. 2000. Pen­guins use the two-voice sys­tem to rec­og­nize each other. Pro­ceed­ings of the Royal So­ci­ety B: Bi­o­log­i­cal Sci­ences, 267: 1081-1087.

Bax­ter, W. 1974. Peo­ple or Pen­guins: The Case for Op­ti­mal Pol­lu­tion. New York: Co­lum­bia Uni­ver­sity Press.

Bried, J., F. Jiguet, P. Jou­ventin. 1999. Why do Apten­odytes pen­guins have high di­vorce rates?. The Auk, 116: 504-512.

Char­bon­nier, Y., K. De­lord, J. Thiebot. 2009. King-size fast food for An­tar­tic fur seals. Polar Bi­ol­ogy, 33: 721-724.

Cote, S. 2000. Ag­gres­sive­ness in king pen­guins in re­la­tion to re­pro­duc­tive sta­tus and ter­ri­tory lo­ca­tion. An­i­mal Be­hav­iour, 59: 813-821.

Dob­son, F., P. Jou­ventin. 2003. Use of the nest site as a ren­dezvous in pen­guins. Wa­ter­birds: The In­ter­na­tional Jour­nal of Wa­ter­bird Bi­ol­ogy, 26: 409-415.

Em­slie, S., N. Karnovsky, W. Triv­el­piece. 1995. Avian pre­da­tion at pen­guin colonies on King George Is­land, Antarc­tica. The Wil­son Bul­letin, 107: 317-327.

Flower, 1938. Furter notes on the du­ra­tion of life in an­i­mals. IV. Birds. Pro­ceed­ings of the Zo­o­log­i­cal So­ci­ety of Lon­don, Ser. A: 195-235.

Gau­thier-Clerc, M., Y. Clerquin, Y. Han­drich. 1998. Hy­per­in­fes­ta­tion by ticks Ixodes uriae: a pos­si­ble cause of death in adult king pen­guins, a long-lived seabird. Colo­nial Wa­ter­birds, 21: 229-233.

Jou­ventin, P., T. Aubin, T. Lengagne. 1999. Find­ing a par­ent in a king pen­guin colony: the acoustic sys­tem of in­di­vid­ual recog­ni­tion. An­i­mal Be­hav­ior, 57: 1175-1183.

Jou­ventin, P., P. Nolan, J. Orn­borg, F. Dob­son. 2005. Ul­tra­vi­o­let beak spots in king and em­peror pen­guins. The Con­dor, 107: 144-150.

Kooy­man, G., Y. Cherel, Y. Le Maho, J. Crox­all, P. Thor­son, V. Ri­doux, C. Kooy­man. 1992. Div­ing be­hav­ior and en­er­get­ics dur­ing for­ag­ing cy­cles in king pen­guins. Eco­log­i­cal Mono­graphs, 62: 143-163.

Lock­ley, R. 1984. Seabirds of the World. New York, NY: Facts on File.

Mc­Go­ni­gal, D., L. Wood­worth. 2001. Antarc­tica and the Arc­tic: The Com­plete En­cy­clo­pe­dia. On­tario: Fire­fly Books.

Nel­son, B. 1979. Seabirds, Their Bi­ol­ogy and Ecol­ogy. New York, NY: A & W Pub­lish­ers Inc..

Nico­laus, M., C. Le Bohec, P. Nolan, M. Gau­thier-Clerc, Y. Le Maho, J. Komdeur, P. Jou­ventin. 2007. Or­na­men­tal col­ors re­veal age in the king pen­guin. Polar Bi­ol­ogy, 31: 53-61.

Nolan, P., F. Dob­son, M. Nico­laus, T. Karels, K. Mc­Graw, P. Jou­ventin. 2010. Mu­tual mate choice for col­or­ful traits in king pen­guins. Ethol­ogy, 116: 635-644.

Ols­son, O. 1997. Clutch aban­don­ment: a state-de­pen­dent de­ci­sion in king pen­guins. Jour­nal of Avian Bi­ol­ogy, 28: 264-267.

Ols­son, O. 1996. Sea­sonal ef­fects of tim­ing and re­pro­duc­tion in the king pen­guin: a unique breed­ing cycle. Jour­nal of Avian Bi­ol­ogy, 27: 7-14.

Otley, H. 2005. Na­ture-based tourism: Ex­pe­ri­ences at the vol­un­teer point pen­guin colony in the Falk­land Is­lands. Ma­rine Or­nithol­ogy, 33: 181-187.

Parmelee, D. 1992. Antarc­tic Birds: Eco­log­i­cal and Be­hav­ioral Ap­proaches. Min­neapo­lis: Uni­ver­sity of Minnnesota Press.

Putz, K., C. Bost. 1994. Feed­ing be­hav­ior of free-rang­ing king pen­guins (Apten­odytes patag­o­ni­cus). Ecol­ogy, 75: 489-497.

Rich­dale, L. 1951. Sex­ual Be­hav­ior in Pen­guins. Lawrence: Uni­ver­sity of Kansas Press.

Shir­i­hai, H. 2008. The Com­plete Guide to Antarc­tic Wildlife: Birds and Ma­rine Mam­mals of the Antarc­tic Con­ti­nent and the South­ern Ocean. Prince­ton, NJ: Prince­ton Uni­ver­sity Press.

Viera, V., P. Nolan, S. Cote, P. Jou­ventin, R. Grosco­las. 2007. Is ter­ri­tory de­fence re­lated to plumage or­na­ments in the king pen­guin Apten­odytes patag­o­ni­cus?. Ethol­ogy, 114: 146-153.

de Vil­liers, M., P. de Bruyn. 2004. Stone-swal­low­ing by three species of pen­guins at sub-antarc­tic Mar­ion Is­land. Ma­rine Or­nithol­ogy, 32: 185-186.